Phonetica was published by Karger Publishers up to and including 2020. If you or your institution subscribed to Phonetica during that period, you might still have access to the full text of this article on the Karger platform if you cannot access it here.
Abstract
The prosodic structure of under-researched languages in the Trade Malay language family is poorly understood. Although boundary marking has been uncontroversially shown as the major prosodic function in these languages, studies on the use of pitch accents to highlight important words in a phrase remain inconclusive. In addition, most knowledge of pitch accents is based on well-researched languages such as the ones from the Western-Germanic language family. This paper reports two word identification experiments comparing Papuan Malay with the pitch accent language American English, in order to investigate the extent to which the demarcating and highlighting function of prosody can be disentangled. To this end, target words were presented to native listeners of both languages and differed with respect to their position in the phrase (medial or final) and the shape of their f0 movement (original or manipulated). Reaction times for the target word identifications revealed overall faster responses for original and final words compared to manipulated and medial ones. The results add to previous findings on the facilitating effect of pitch accents and further improve our prosodic knowledge of underresearched languages.
Acknowledgements
Research for this paper was funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG) – Project-ID 281511265 – SFB 1252. The authors thank the staff of the Center for Endangered Languages Documentation (CELD, Manokwari, West-Papua) and Bob Kennedy (Department of Linguistics at the University of California, Santa Barbara; UCSB) for facilitating the Papuan Malay and American English experiments respectively and for participant recruitment. The authors are furthermore grateful to Michael Fiddler for providing the American English materials and to two anonymous reviewers for valuable comments on an earlier version of this paper.
-
Research funding: The research for this paper has been funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) – Project-ID 281511265 – SFB 1252 Prominence in Language.
-
Author contributions: CK: concept and design, experimental setup, running PM experiment, analysis and interpretation of data, drafting and revising manuscript. MG: recording AE stimuli, running AE experiment, interpretation of data, revising manuscript.
-
Conflict of interest statement: The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
-
Statement of ethics: The experiments reported in this paper have been conducted following protocols and informed consent practices in compliance with the Helsinki Declaration. Prior approval of the protocols and informed consent procedures was granted by the CELD and the human subject committee of the UCSB for the Papuan Malay and American English experiment respectively (Project Number: 55, Keycode: LING-GO-MA-007, Protocol Number: 55-21-0302).
Target and distractor words in Papuan Malay and English as used in the different phrase positions in the experiments.
Position | Papuan Malay | American English | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Target | Gloss | Distractor | Gloss | Target | Distractor | |
medial | jata | allotment | kita | 1pl | llama | puma |
laki | husband | hoki | plant stem | really | holy | |
ruma | house | lama | to be long (of duration) | needy | moody | |
tali | cord | tuli | to be deaf | taller | teller | |
ribu | thousand | rubu | to collapse | roller | ruler | |
satu | one | situ | l.med | sully | silly | |
tiga | three | juga | also | fuzzy | dizzy | |
tuju | seven | maju | to advance | fishy | bushy | |
buru | to hunt | baru | to be new | burrow | borrow | |
butu | to need | jatu | to fall | lucky | rocky | |
kira | to think | tara | to be matching | psycho | taco | |
luru | to chase after | biru | to be blue | worry | berry | |
pili | to choose | kali | river | tuna | china | |
pisa | to be separate | hosa | to pant | polo | cello | |
suka | to enjoy | nika | to marry officially | silly | Jolly | |
bisa | to be able | basa | to be wet | butter | batter | |
gila | to be crazy | mala | even | ditto | ghetto | |
kaco | to tell off | koco | to tell off | petty | pity | |
mara | to feel angry | mera | to be red | moody | muddy | |
lusa | day after tomorrow | masa | to be impossible | limo | memo | |
final | besi | metal | nasi | cooked rice | body | lady |
duri | thorn | diri | self | belly | bully | |
gaba | unhulled paddy | tiba | to arrive | baggy | foggy | |
gigi | tooth | pagi | morning | beady | tidy | |
gora | water apple | gara | to irritate | runny | rainy | |
lida | tongue | lada | pepper | lousy | lazy | |
mace | woman | cece | great-grandchild | naggy | piggy | |
mati | to die | meti | low tide | pillow | polo | |
paku | nail | suku | ethnic group | kitty | fatty | |
puri | anchovy-like fish | kiri | left | picky | caky | |
rawa | swamp | kewa | dance party | buggy | soggy | |
sala | to be wrong | hela | to haul | kilo | Halo | |
subu | very early morning | tubu | body | fallow | shallow | |
tipu | to cheat | tepu | to clap | phoney | funny | |
tugu | monument | lagu | song | sorry | marry | |
bera | to defecate | para | to be in serious condition | data | pita | |
cebo | to wash after defecating | bobo | nipah palm fruit schnapps | sumo | limo | |
gale | to dig up | bule | white person | nutty | meaty | |
taru | to put | tiru | to imitate | curry | carry | |
malu | to feel embarrassed | bulu | body hair | mono | rhino |
References
Arnhold, Anja. 2014. Prosodic structure and focus realization in West Greenlandic. In Sun-Ah Jun (ed.), Prosodic typology II, 216–251. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199567300.003.0008Search in Google Scholar
Baayen, Harald R. & Petar Milin. 2010. Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research 3(2). 12–28. https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.807.Search in Google Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Martin Mächler, Bolker Ben & Steve Walker. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67(1). 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.Search in Google Scholar
Blevins, Juliette. 2004. Evolutionary phonology: The emergence of sound patterns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511486357.Search in Google Scholar
Blevins, Juliette. 2006. A theoretical synopsis of Evolutionary Phonology. Theoretical Linguistics 32(2). 117–166. https://doi.org/10.1515/TL.2006.009.Search in Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul & David Weenink. 2019. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer. Available at: http://www.praat.org/.Search in Google Scholar
Cole, Jennifer & Stefanie Shattuck-Hufnagel. 2016. New methods for prosodic transcription: Capturing variability as a source of information. Laboratory Phonology 7(1). 1–29. https://doi.org/10.5334/labphon.29.Search in Google Scholar
Cutler, Anne. 2012. Native listening: Language experience and the recognition of spoken words. OCLC: 816479318. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/9012.001.0001Search in Google Scholar
Cutler, Anne & Jerry A. Fodor. 1979. Semantic focus and sentence comprehension. Cognition 7(1). 49–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(79)90010-6.Search in Google Scholar
Cutler, Anne & Donald J. Foss. 1977. On the role of sentence stress in sentence processing. Language and Speech 20(1). 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383097702000101.Search in Google Scholar
De Pijper, J. Roelof. 1983. Modelling British English intonation: An analysis by resynthesis of British English intonation. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.10.1515/9783110883510Search in Google Scholar
Evans, James D. 1996. Straightforward statistics for the behavioral sciences. English. Pacific Grove: Duxbury Press.Search in Google Scholar
Fernald, Anne & Claudia Mazzie. 1991. Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology 27(2). 209–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.209.Search in Google Scholar
Fery, Caroline. 2010. Indian languages as intonational ’phrase languages. In S. Imtiaz Hasnain & Shreesh Chaudhary (eds.), Problematizing language studies: Cultural, theoretical and applied perspectives—Essays in honor of Rama Kant Agnihotri, 288–312. Delhi: Aakar Books.Search in Google Scholar
Foss, Donald J. 1969. Decision processes during sentence comprehension: Effects of lexical item difficulty and position upon decision times. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 8(4). 457–462. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(69)80089-7.Search in Google Scholar
Fox, John & Sanford Weisberg. 2019. An R companion to applied regression, 3rd edn. Thousand Oaks CA: Sage.Search in Google Scholar
Frazier, Lyn, Katy Carlson & Charles Cliftonjr. 2006. Prosodic phrasing is central to language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(6). 244–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.04.002.Search in Google Scholar
Gordon, Matthew. 2014. Disentangling stress and pitch-accent: A typology of prominence at different prosodic levels. In Harry vander Hulst (ed.), Word stress, 83–118. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139600408.005Search in Google Scholar
Gordon, Matthew & Timo Roettger. 2017. Acoustic correlates of word stress: A cross-linguistic survey. Linguistics Vanguard 3(1). 20170007. https://doi.org/10.1515/lingvan-2017-0007.Search in Google Scholar
Henrich, Joseph, Steven J. Heine & Ara Norenzayan. 2010. The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33(2–3). 61–83. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x0999152x.Search in Google Scholar
Hilbig, Benjamin E. 2016. Reaction time effects in lab- versus Web-based research: Experimental evidence. Behavior Research Methods 48(4). 1718–1724. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0678-9.Search in Google Scholar
Hillenbrand, James M. 2003. Some effects of intonation contour on sentence intelligibility. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 114(4). 2338. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4781079.Search in Google Scholar
Himmelmann, Nikolaus P. & D. Robert Ladd. 2008. Prosodic description: An introduction for fieldworkers. Language Documentation & Conservation 2(2). 244–274.Search in Google Scholar
Himmelmann, Nikolaus P., Meytal Sandler, Strunk Jan & Volker Unterladstetter. 2018. On the universality of intonational phrases: A cross-linguistic interrater study. Phonology 35(2). 207–245. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675718000039.Search in Google Scholar
James, Gareth, Daniela Witten, Trevor Hastie & Robert Tibshirani. 2013. An introduction to statistical learning, vol. 103 (Springer Texts in Statistics). New York, NY: Springer New York.10.1007/978-1-4614-7138-7Search in Google Scholar
Jun, Sun-Ah (ed.). 2005 Prosodic typology: The phonology of intonation and phrasing (Oxford linguistics). Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199249633.001.0001Search in Google Scholar
Jun, Sun-Ah (ed.). 2014 Prosodictypology II: The phonology of intonation and phrasing (Oxford linguistics). Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199567300.001.0001Search in Google Scholar
Jun, Sun-Ah & Cécile Fougeron. 2002. Realizations of accentual phrase in French intonation. Probus 14(1). 147–172. https://doi.org/10.1515/prbs.2002.002.Search in Google Scholar
Kaland, Constantijn. 2019. Acoustic correlates of word stress in Papuan Malay. Journal of Phonetics 74. 55–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2019.02.003.Search in Google Scholar
Kaland, Constantijn. 2020. Offline and online processing of acoustic cues to word stress in Papuan Malay. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 147(2). 731–747. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0000578.Search in Google Scholar
Kaland, Constantijn & Stefan Baumann. 2020. Demarcating and highlighting in Papuan Malay phrase prosody. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 147(4). 2974–2988. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0001008.Search in Google Scholar
Kaland, Constantijn & Nikolaus P. Himmelmann. 2020. Time-series analysis of F0 in Papuan Malay contrastive focus. In Proceedings of the 10th international conference on speech prosody 2020, 230–234. Tokyo: University of Tokyo.10.21437/SpeechProsody.2020-47Search in Google Scholar
Karlsson, Anastasia M. 2014. The intonational phonology of Mongolian. In Sun-Ah Jun (ed.), Prosodic typology II, 187–215. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199567300.003.0007Search in Google Scholar
Kaufman, Daniel & Nikolaus P. Himmelmann. n.d. Suprasegmental phonology. In Adelaar Alexander & Antoinette Schapper (eds.), The Oxford guide to the western Austronesian languages. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Kim, Jonathan, Ute Gabriel & Gygax Pascal. 2019. Testing the effectiveness of the internet- based instrument PsyToolkit: A comparison between web-based (PsyToolkit) and lab-based (e-Prime3.0) measurements of response choice and response time in a complex psycholinguistic task. PLoS One 14(9). e0221802. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221802.Search in Google Scholar
Kluge, Angela. 2017. A grammar of Papuan Malay. Berlin, Germany: Language Science Press.Search in Google Scholar
Kluge, Angela, Benny Rumaropen & Lodowik Aweta. 2014. Papuan Malaydata-Wordlist. Dallas, TX: SIL International.Search in Google Scholar
Krinzinger, Helga, Jan Willem Koten, Julia Hennemann, André Schueppen, Katleen Sahr, Dominique Arndt, Kerstin Konrad & Klaus Willmes. 2011. Sensitivity, reproducibility, and reliability of self-paced versus fixed stimulus presentation in an fMRI study on exact, non-symbolic arithmeticin typically developing children aged 664 between 6 and 12 years. Developmental Neuropsychology 36(6). 721–740. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2010.549882.Search in Google Scholar
Ladd, D. Robert. 2008. Intonational phonology, 2nd edn. (Cambridge studies in linguistics). Cambridge, NewYork: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511808814Search in Google Scholar
Laures, Jacqueline S. & Gary Weismer. 1999. The effects of a flattened fundamental frequency on intelligibility at the sentence level. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42(5). 1148–1156. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4205.1148.Search in Google Scholar
Maskikit-Essed, Raechel & Carlos Gussenhoven. 2016. No stress, nopitchaccent, noprosodic focus: The case of ambonese Malay. Phonology 33(2). 353–389. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675716000154.Search in Google Scholar
Mathôt, Sebastiaan, Daniel Schreij & Theeuwes Jan. 2012. Open Sesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods 44(2). 314–324. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7.Search in Google Scholar
Mehta, Gita & Anne Cutler. 1988. Detection of target phonemes in spontaneous and read speech. Language and Speech 31(2). 135–156. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383098803100203.Search in Google Scholar
Mixdorff, Hansjörg & Dieter Mehnert. 1999. Exploring the naturalness of several German high-quality-text-to-speech systems. In Sixth European Conference on speech communication and technology. Budapest: International Speech Communication Association.10.21437/Eurospeech.1999-406Search in Google Scholar
Moulines, Eric & Francis Charpentier. 1990. Pitch-synchronous wave form processing techniques for text-to-speech synthesis using diphones. Speech Communication. Neuropeech ’89 9(5). 453–467. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-6393(90)90021-Z.Search in Google Scholar
Özçelik, Öner. 2012. Redefining the prosodic hierarchy. McGill Working Papers in Linguistics 22(1). 1–13.Search in Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet & Julia Hirschberg. 1990. The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse. In Philip R. Cohen, Jerry Morgan & Martha E. Pollack (eds.), Intentions in communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7916/d8kd24fp.Search in Google Scholar
R Core Team. 2019. R: The R project for statistical computing.Search in Google Scholar
Rietveld, Toni & Carlos Gussenhoven. 1987. Perceived speech rate and intonation. Journal of Phonetics 15(3). 273–285. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(19)30571-6.Search in Google Scholar
Shattuck-Hufnagel, Stefanie & Alice Turk. 1998. The domain of phrase-final lengthening in English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 103(5). 2889. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.421798.Search in Google Scholar
Shields, Joyce L., Astrid McHugh & James G. Martin. 1974. Reaction time to phoneme targets as a function of rhythmic cues in continuous speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology 102(2). 250–255. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035855.Search in Google Scholar
Silverman, Kim, Mary Beckman, John Pitrelli, Ostendorf Mori, Colin Wightman, Patti Price, Janet Pierrehumbert & Julia Hirschberg. 1992. ToBI: A standard for labeling English prosody. In Second international conference on spoken language processing. Banff: International Speech Communication Association.10.21437/ICSLP.1992-260Search in Google Scholar
Slote, Joseph & Julia F. Strand. 2016. Conducting spoken word recognition research online: Validation and a new timing method. Behavior Research Methods 48(2). 553–566. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0599-7.Search in Google Scholar
Stoel, R. B. 2007. The intonation of Manado Malay. In Vincent, J. & Ellen van Zanten (eds.), Prosody in Indonesian languages. vol. 9 (LOT occasional series), 117–150. Utrecht: LOT, Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar
Stoet, Gijsbert. 2010. PsyToolkit: A software package for programming psychological experiments using linux. Behavior Research Methods 42(4). 1096–1104. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.4.1096.Search in Google Scholar
Stoet, Gijsbert. 2017. PsyToolkit: A novel web-based method for running online questionnaires and reaction-time experiments. Teaching of Psychology 44(1). 24–31. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628316677643.Search in Google Scholar
Taft, Marcus & Gail Hambly. 1986. Exploring the cohort model of spoken word recognition. Cognition 22(3). 259–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(86)90017-X.Search in Google Scholar
Van Rossum, Guido & Jelke De Boer. 1991. Interactively testing remote servers using the Python programming language. CWI Quarterly 4(4). 283–304.Search in Google Scholar
Watson, Charles S., Henry W. Wroton, William J. Kelly & Carole A. Benbassat. 1975. Factors in the discrimination of tonal patterns. I. Component frequency, temporal position, and silent intervals. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 57(5). 1175–1185. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.380576.Search in Google Scholar
© 2022 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston