The role of f0 shape and phrasal position in Papuan Malay and American English word identification Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter Mouton August 19, 2022

The role of f0 shape and phrasal position in Papuan Malay and American English word identification

  • Constantijn Kaland EMAIL logo and Matthew K. Gordon
From the journal Phonetica

Abstract

The prosodic structure of under-researched languages in the Trade Malay language family is poorly understood. Although boundary marking has been uncontroversially shown as the major prosodic function in these languages, studies on the use of pitch accents to highlight important words in a phrase remain inconclusive. In addition, most knowledge of pitch accents is based on well-researched languages such as the ones from the Western-Germanic language family. This paper reports two word identification experiments comparing Papuan Malay with the pitch accent language American English, in order to investigate the extent to which the demarcating and highlighting function of prosody can be disentangled. To this end, target words were presented to native listeners of both languages and differed with respect to their position in the phrase (medial or final) and the shape of their f0 movement (original or manipulated). Reaction times for the target word identifications revealed overall faster responses for original and final words compared to manipulated and medial ones. The results add to previous findings on the facilitating effect of pitch accents and further improve our prosodic knowledge of underresearched languages.


Corresponding author: Constantijn Kaland, Institute of Linguistics, Universität zu Köln – SFB 1252, Luxemburger Straße 299, 50939 Cologne, Germany, E-mail:

Acknowledgements

Research for this paper was funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG) – Project-ID 281511265 – SFB 1252. The authors thank the staff of the Center for Endangered Languages Documentation (CELD, Manokwari, West-Papua) and Bob Kennedy (Department of Linguistics at the University of California, Santa Barbara; UCSB) for facilitating the Papuan Malay and American English experiments respectively and for participant recruitment. The authors are furthermore grateful to Michael Fiddler for providing the American English materials and to two anonymous reviewers for valuable comments on an earlier version of this paper.

  1. Research funding: The research for this paper has been funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) – Project-ID 281511265 – SFB 1252 Prominence in Language.

  2. Author contributions: CK: concept and design, experimental setup, running PM experiment, analysis and interpretation of data, drafting and revising manuscript. MG: recording AE stimuli, running AE experiment, interpretation of data, revising manuscript.

  3. Conflict of interest statement: The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

  4. Statement of ethics: The experiments reported in this paper have been conducted following protocols and informed consent practices in compliance with the Helsinki Declaration. Prior approval of the protocols and informed consent procedures was granted by the CELD and the human subject committee of the UCSB for the Papuan Malay and American English experiment respectively (Project Number: 55, Keycode: LING-GO-MA-007, Protocol Number: 55-21-0302).

Appendix A

Target and distractor words in Papuan Malay and English as used in the different phrase positions in the experiments.

Position Papuan Malay American English
Target Gloss Distractor Gloss Target Distractor
medial jata allotment kita 1pl llama puma
laki husband hoki plant stem really holy
ruma house lama to be long (of duration) needy moody
tali cord tuli to be deaf taller teller
ribu thousand rubu to collapse roller ruler
satu one situ l.med sully silly
tiga three juga also fuzzy dizzy
tuju seven maju to advance fishy bushy
buru to hunt baru to be new burrow borrow
butu to need jatu to fall lucky rocky
kira to think tara to be matching psycho taco
luru to chase after biru to be blue worry berry
pili to choose kali river tuna china
pisa to be separate hosa to pant polo cello
suka to enjoy nika to marry officially silly Jolly
bisa to be able basa to be wet butter batter
gila to be crazy mala even ditto ghetto
kaco to tell off koco to tell off petty pity
mara to feel angry mera to be red moody muddy
lusa day after tomorrow masa to be impossible limo memo
final besi metal nasi cooked rice body lady
duri thorn diri self belly bully
gaba unhulled paddy tiba to arrive baggy foggy
gigi tooth pagi morning beady tidy
gora water apple gara to irritate runny rainy
lida tongue lada pepper lousy lazy
mace woman cece great-grandchild naggy piggy
mati to die meti low tide pillow polo
paku nail suku ethnic group kitty fatty
puri anchovy-like fish kiri left picky caky
rawa swamp kewa dance party buggy soggy
sala to be wrong hela to haul kilo Halo
subu very early morning tubu body fallow shallow
tipu to cheat tepu to clap phoney funny
tugu monument lagu song sorry marry
bera to defecate para to be in serious condition data pita
cebo to wash after defecating bobo nipah palm fruit schnapps sumo limo
gale to dig up bule white person nutty meaty
taru to put tiru to imitate curry carry
malu to feel embarrassed bulu body hair mono rhino

References

Arnhold, Anja. 2014. Prosodic structure and focus realization in West Greenlandic. In Sun-Ah Jun (ed.), Prosodic typology II, 216–251. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199567300.003.0008Search in Google Scholar

Baayen, Harald R. & Petar Milin. 2010. Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research 3(2). 12–28. https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.807.Search in Google Scholar

Bates, Douglas, Martin Mächler, Bolker Ben & Steve Walker. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67(1). 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.Search in Google Scholar

Blevins, Juliette. 2004. Evolutionary phonology: The emergence of sound patterns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511486357.Search in Google Scholar

Blevins, Juliette. 2006. A theoretical synopsis of Evolutionary Phonology. Theoretical Linguistics 32(2). 117–166. https://doi.org/10.1515/TL.2006.009.Search in Google Scholar

Boersma, Paul & David Weenink. 2019. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer. Available at: http://www.praat.org/.Search in Google Scholar

Cole, Jennifer & Stefanie Shattuck-Hufnagel. 2016. New methods for prosodic transcription: Capturing variability as a source of information. Laboratory Phonology 7(1). 1–29. https://doi.org/10.5334/labphon.29.Search in Google Scholar

Cutler, Anne. 2012. Native listening: Language experience and the recognition of spoken words. OCLC: 816479318. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/9012.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Cutler, Anne & Jerry A. Fodor. 1979. Semantic focus and sentence comprehension. Cognition 7(1). 49–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(79)90010-6.Search in Google Scholar

Cutler, Anne & Donald J. Foss. 1977. On the role of sentence stress in sentence processing. Language and Speech 20(1). 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383097702000101.Search in Google Scholar

De Pijper, J. Roelof. 1983. Modelling British English intonation: An analysis by resynthesis of British English intonation. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.10.1515/9783110883510Search in Google Scholar

Evans, James D. 1996. Straightforward statistics for the behavioral sciences. English. Pacific Grove: Duxbury Press.Search in Google Scholar

Fernald, Anne & Claudia Mazzie. 1991. Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology 27(2). 209–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.209.Search in Google Scholar

Fery, Caroline. 2010. Indian languages as intonational ’phrase languages. In S. Imtiaz Hasnain & Shreesh Chaudhary (eds.), Problematizing language studies: Cultural, theoretical and applied perspectives—Essays in honor of Rama Kant Agnihotri, 288–312. Delhi: Aakar Books.Search in Google Scholar

Foss, Donald J. 1969. Decision processes during sentence comprehension: Effects of lexical item difficulty and position upon decision times. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 8(4). 457–462. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(69)80089-7.Search in Google Scholar

Fox, John & Sanford Weisberg. 2019. An R companion to applied regression, 3rd edn. Thousand Oaks CA: Sage.Search in Google Scholar

Frazier, Lyn, Katy Carlson & Charles Cliftonjr. 2006. Prosodic phrasing is central to language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(6). 244–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.04.002.Search in Google Scholar

Gordon, Matthew. 2014. Disentangling stress and pitch-accent: A typology of prominence at different prosodic levels. In Harry vander Hulst (ed.), Word stress, 83–118. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139600408.005Search in Google Scholar

Gordon, Matthew & Timo Roettger. 2017. Acoustic correlates of word stress: A cross-linguistic survey. Linguistics Vanguard 3(1). 20170007. https://doi.org/10.1515/lingvan-2017-0007.Search in Google Scholar

Henrich, Joseph, Steven J. Heine & Ara Norenzayan. 2010. The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33(2–3). 61–83. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x0999152x.Search in Google Scholar

Hilbig, Benjamin E. 2016. Reaction time effects in lab- versus Web-based research: Experimental evidence. Behavior Research Methods 48(4). 1718–1724. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0678-9.Search in Google Scholar

Hillenbrand, James M. 2003. Some effects of intonation contour on sentence intelligibility. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 114(4). 2338. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4781079.Search in Google Scholar

Himmelmann, Nikolaus P. & D. Robert Ladd. 2008. Prosodic description: An introduction for fieldworkers. Language Documentation & Conservation 2(2). 244–274.Search in Google Scholar

Himmelmann, Nikolaus P., Meytal Sandler, Strunk Jan & Volker Unterladstetter. 2018. On the universality of intonational phrases: A cross-linguistic interrater study. Phonology 35(2). 207–245. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675718000039.Search in Google Scholar

James, Gareth, Daniela Witten, Trevor Hastie & Robert Tibshirani. 2013. An introduction to statistical learning, vol. 103 (Springer Texts in Statistics). New York, NY: Springer New York.10.1007/978-1-4614-7138-7Search in Google Scholar

Jun, Sun-Ah (ed.). 2005 Prosodic typology: The phonology of intonation and phrasing (Oxford linguistics). Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199249633.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Jun, Sun-Ah (ed.). 2014 Prosodictypology II: The phonology of intonation and phrasing (Oxford linguistics). Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199567300.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Jun, Sun-Ah & Cécile Fougeron. 2002. Realizations of accentual phrase in French intonation. Probus 14(1). 147–172. https://doi.org/10.1515/prbs.2002.002.Search in Google Scholar

Kaland, Constantijn. 2019. Acoustic correlates of word stress in Papuan Malay. Journal of Phonetics 74. 55–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2019.02.003.Search in Google Scholar

Kaland, Constantijn. 2020. Offline and online processing of acoustic cues to word stress in Papuan Malay. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 147(2). 731–747. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0000578.Search in Google Scholar

Kaland, Constantijn & Stefan Baumann. 2020. Demarcating and highlighting in Papuan Malay phrase prosody. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 147(4). 2974–2988. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0001008.Search in Google Scholar

Kaland, Constantijn & Nikolaus P. Himmelmann. 2020. Time-series analysis of F0 in Papuan Malay contrastive focus. In Proceedings of the 10th international conference on speech prosody 2020, 230–234. Tokyo: University of Tokyo.10.21437/SpeechProsody.2020-47Search in Google Scholar

Karlsson, Anastasia M. 2014. The intonational phonology of Mongolian. In Sun-Ah Jun (ed.), Prosodic typology II, 187–215. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199567300.003.0007Search in Google Scholar

Kaufman, Daniel & Nikolaus P. Himmelmann. n.d. Suprasegmental phonology. In Adelaar Alexander & Antoinette Schapper (eds.), The Oxford guide to the western Austronesian languages. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Kim, Jonathan, Ute Gabriel & Gygax Pascal. 2019. Testing the effectiveness of the internet- based instrument PsyToolkit: A comparison between web-based (PsyToolkit) and lab-based (e-Prime3.0) measurements of response choice and response time in a complex psycholinguistic task. PLoS One 14(9). e0221802. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221802.Search in Google Scholar

Kluge, Angela. 2017. A grammar of Papuan Malay. Berlin, Germany: Language Science Press.Search in Google Scholar

Kluge, Angela, Benny Rumaropen & Lodowik Aweta. 2014. Papuan Malaydata-Wordlist. Dallas, TX: SIL International.Search in Google Scholar

Krinzinger, Helga, Jan Willem Koten, Julia Hennemann, André Schueppen, Katleen Sahr, Dominique Arndt, Kerstin Konrad & Klaus Willmes. 2011. Sensitivity, reproducibility, and reliability of self-paced versus fixed stimulus presentation in an fMRI study on exact, non-symbolic arithmeticin typically developing children aged 664 between 6 and 12 years. Developmental Neuropsychology 36(6). 721–740. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2010.549882.Search in Google Scholar

Ladd, D. Robert. 2008. Intonational phonology, 2nd edn. (Cambridge studies in linguistics). Cambridge, NewYork: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511808814Search in Google Scholar

Laures, Jacqueline S. & Gary Weismer. 1999. The effects of a flattened fundamental frequency on intelligibility at the sentence level. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42(5). 1148–1156. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4205.1148.Search in Google Scholar

Maskikit-Essed, Raechel & Carlos Gussenhoven. 2016. No stress, nopitchaccent, noprosodic focus: The case of ambonese Malay. Phonology 33(2). 353–389. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675716000154.Search in Google Scholar

Mathôt, Sebastiaan, Daniel Schreij & Theeuwes Jan. 2012. Open Sesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods 44(2). 314–324. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7.Search in Google Scholar

Mehta, Gita & Anne Cutler. 1988. Detection of target phonemes in spontaneous and read speech. Language and Speech 31(2). 135–156. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383098803100203.Search in Google Scholar

Mixdorff, Hansjörg & Dieter Mehnert. 1999. Exploring the naturalness of several German high-quality-text-to-speech systems. In Sixth European Conference on speech communication and technology. Budapest: International Speech Communication Association.10.21437/Eurospeech.1999-406Search in Google Scholar

Moulines, Eric & Francis Charpentier. 1990. Pitch-synchronous wave form processing techniques for text-to-speech synthesis using diphones. Speech Communication. Neuropeech ’89 9(5). 453–467. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-6393(90)90021-Z.Search in Google Scholar

Özçelik, Öner. 2012. Redefining the prosodic hierarchy. McGill Working Papers in Linguistics 22(1). 1–13.Search in Google Scholar

Pierrehumbert, Janet & Julia Hirschberg. 1990. The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse. In Philip R. Cohen, Jerry Morgan & Martha E. Pollack (eds.), Intentions in communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7916/d8kd24fp.Search in Google Scholar

R Core Team. 2019. R: The R project for statistical computing.Search in Google Scholar

Rietveld, Toni & Carlos Gussenhoven. 1987. Perceived speech rate and intonation. Journal of Phonetics 15(3). 273–285. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(19)30571-6.Search in Google Scholar

Shattuck-Hufnagel, Stefanie & Alice Turk. 1998. The domain of phrase-final lengthening in English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 103(5). 2889. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.421798.Search in Google Scholar

Shields, Joyce L., Astrid McHugh & James G. Martin. 1974. Reaction time to phoneme targets as a function of rhythmic cues in continuous speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology 102(2). 250–255. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035855.Search in Google Scholar

Silverman, Kim, Mary Beckman, John Pitrelli, Ostendorf Mori, Colin Wightman, Patti Price, Janet Pierrehumbert & Julia Hirschberg. 1992. ToBI: A standard for labeling English prosody. In Second international conference on spoken language processing. Banff: International Speech Communication Association.10.21437/ICSLP.1992-260Search in Google Scholar

Slote, Joseph & Julia F. Strand. 2016. Conducting spoken word recognition research online: Validation and a new timing method. Behavior Research Methods 48(2). 553–566. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0599-7.Search in Google Scholar

Stoel, R. B. 2007. The intonation of Manado Malay. In Vincent, J. & Ellen van Zanten (eds.), Prosody in Indonesian languages. vol. 9 (LOT occasional series), 117–150. Utrecht: LOT, Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Stoet, Gijsbert. 2010. PsyToolkit: A software package for programming psychological experiments using linux. Behavior Research Methods 42(4). 1096–1104. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.4.1096.Search in Google Scholar

Stoet, Gijsbert. 2017. PsyToolkit: A novel web-based method for running online questionnaires and reaction-time experiments. Teaching of Psychology 44(1). 24–31. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628316677643.Search in Google Scholar

Taft, Marcus & Gail Hambly. 1986. Exploring the cohort model of spoken word recognition. Cognition 22(3). 259–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(86)90017-X.Search in Google Scholar

Van Rossum, Guido & Jelke De Boer. 1991. Interactively testing remote servers using the Python programming language. CWI Quarterly 4(4). 283–304.Search in Google Scholar

Watson, Charles S., Henry W. Wroton, William J. Kelly & Carole A. Benbassat. 1975. Factors in the discrimination of tonal patterns. I. Component frequency, temporal position, and silent intervals. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 57(5). 1175–1185. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.380576.Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2022-08-19
Published in Print: 2022-06-27

© 2022 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 22.3.2025 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/phon-2022-2022/html
Scroll to top button