Abstract
As one of the best known science narratives about the consequences of creating life, Mary Shelley’s Frankenstein; or, The Modern Prometheus (1818) is an enduring tale that people know and understand with an almost instinctive familiarity. It has become a myth reflecting people’s ambivalent feelings about emerging science: they are curious about science, but they are also afraid of what science can do to them. In this essay, we argue that the Frankenstein myth has evolved into a stigma attached to scientists that focalizes the public’s as well as the scientific community’s negative reactions towards certain sciences and scientific practices. This stigma produces ambivalent reactions towards scientific artifacts and it leads to negative connotations because it implies that some sciences are dangerous and harmful. We argue that understanding the Frankenstein stigma can empower scientists by helping them revisit their own biases as well as responding effectively to people’s expectations for, and attitudes towards, scientists and scientific artifacts. Debunking the Frankenstein stigma could also allow scientists to reshape their professional identities so they can better show the public what ethical and moral values guide their research enterprises.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
The authors wish to make clear that we do not, in fact, subscribe to this reading of the narrative. We see Shelley’s novel as a much more nuanced exploration of scientific creativity and responsibility. Nevertheless, the popular conception of Frankenstein, and many of its adaptations, interpret it as a cautionary tale.
Originating from alchemy and nineteenth-century fiction, a homunculus refers to an artificially created miniature human being (Newman 2004).
References
Abraham, J., & Dessler, A. (2013). What scientists should talk about: Their personal stories. The Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/environment/climate-consensus-97-per-cent/2013/sep/20/climate-change-scientists-personal-stories. Accessed October 15, 2016.
Adams, W. W. (2001). Making daemons of death and love: Frankenstein, existentialism, psychoanalysis. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 41(4), 57–89.
Arkin, R. M. (1980). Self-presentation. In D. M. Wegner & R. R. Vallacher (Eds.), The self in social psychology (pp. 158–182). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Arluke, A. (1991). Going into the closet with science: Information control among animal experimenters. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 20(3), 306–330.
Ashforth, B. E., & Kreiner, G. E. (1999). How can you do it? Dirty work and the challenge of constructing a positive identity. Academy of Management Review, 24(3), 413–434.
Barriga, C. A., Shapiro, M. A., & Fernandez, M. L. (2010). Science information in fictional movies: Effects of context and gender. Science Communication, 32(1), 3–24.
Beck, J. (2015). Americans believe in science, just not its findings. The Atlantic. http://www.theatlantic.com/health/archive/2015/01/americans-believe-in-science-just-not-its-findings/384937/. Accessed November 12, 2016.
Bernstein, M. (1997). Celebration and suppression: The strategic uses of identity by the lesbian and gay movement. American Journal of Sociology, 103(3), 531–565.
Bishop, M. G. H. (1994). The “makyng” and re-making of Man: 1. Mary Shelley’s Frankenstein, and transplant surgery. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 87(12), 749–751.
Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., Lickel, B., & Kowai-Bell, N. (2001). Perceiver threat in social interactions with stigmatized others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(2), 253–267.
Bos, A. E. R., Pryor, J. B., Reeder, G. D., & Stutterheim, S. E. (2013). Stigma: Advances in theory and research. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 35(1), 1–9.
Bozeman, B., & Sarewitz, D. (2005). Public values and public failure in US science policy. Science and Public Policy, 32(2), 119–136.
Brotherton, M. (2016). Science fiction by scientists: An anthology of short stories. New York: Springer.
Chambers, D. W. (1983). Stereotypic images of the scientist: The Draw-A-Scientist Test. Science Education, 67(2), 255–265.
Clancy, K. A., & Clancy, B. (2016). Growing monstrous organisms: The construction of anti-GMO visual rhetoric through digital media. Critical Studies in Media Communication. doi:10.1080/15295036.2016.1193670.
Cole, S. (2016). Neuroscientists warn against unsupervised direct brain stimulation—Maybe don’t DIY electrocute your brain. Popular Science. http://www.popsci.com/maybe-dont-diy-electrocute-your-brain. Accessed October 17, 2016.
Corrigan, P. (2004). How stigma interferes with mental health care. American Psychologist, 59(7), 614–625.
Creed, W. E. D., & Scully, M. A. (2000). Songs of ourselves: Employees’ deployment of social identity in workplace encounters. Journal of Management Inquiry, 9(4), 391–412.
Crichton, M. (1999). Ritual abuse, hot air, and missed opportunities. Science, 283(5407), 1461–1463.
Dahlstrom, M. F. (2014). Using narratives and storytelling to communicate science with nonexpert audiences. PNAS, 111(4), 13614–13620.
Dahlstrom, M. F., & Ho, S. S. (2012). Ethical considerations of using narrative to communicate science. Science Communication, 34(5), 592–617.
Dietz, T. (2013). Bringing values and deliberation to science communication. PNAS, 110(3), 14081–14087.
Dolan, L. (2016). Pig-human embryos? ‘Ethically corrupt and belonging in fiction’ says PETA doctor. http://talkradio.co.uk/highlights/pig-human-embryos-ethically-corrupt-and-belonging-fiction-says-peta-doctor-1606061471. Accessed December 18, 2016.
Durkheim, E. [1895] (1982). The rules of sociological method. New York: The Free Press.
Evans, N. G. (2010). Speak no evil: Scientists, responsibility, and the public understanding of science. Nanoethics, 4(3), 215–220.
Feygina, I., Jost, J. T., & Goldsmith, R. E. (2010). System justification, the denial of global warming, and the possibility of “system-sanctioned change”. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(3), 326–338.
Finson, K. D. (2002). Drawing a scientist: What we do and do not know after fifty years of drawings. School Science and Mathematics, 107(7), 335–345.
Fligstein, N. (1997). Social skill and institutional theory. American Behavioral Scientist, 40(4), 397–405.
Flores, G. (2002). Mad scientists, compassionate healers, and greedy egoists: The portrayal of the physicians in the movies. Journal of National Medical Association, 94(7), 635–658.
Franco, D. (1998). Mirror images and otherness in Mary Shelley’s Frankenstein. Literature and Psychology, 44(1–2), 80–95.
Friedman, L. D., & Kavey, A. B. (2016). Monstrous progeny: A history of the Frankenstein narratives. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.
Funk, C., Rainie, L., Smith, A., Olmstead, K., Duggan, M., & Page, D. (2015). Public and scientists’ views on science and society. Pew Research Center. http://www.pewinternet.org/files/2015/01/PI_ScienceandSociety_Report_012915.pdf. Accessed September 15, 2016.
Gauchat, G. (2011). The cultural authority of science: Public trust and acceptance of organized science. Public Understanding of Science, 20(6), 751–770.
Gauchat, G. (2015). The political context of science in the United States: Public acceptance of evidence-based policy and science funding. Social Forces, 94(2), 723–746.
Gerlach, N., & Hamilton, S. N. (2005). From mad scientist to bad scientist: Richard seed as biogovernmental event. Communication Theory, 15(1), 78–99.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma—Notes on the management of spoiled identity. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Goldberg, A. E., & Smith, J. Z. (2011). Stigma, social context, and mental health: Lesbian and gay couples across the transition to adoptive parenthood. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 58(1), 139–150.
Hammond, R. (1986). The modern Frankenstein: Fiction becomes fact. New York: Blandford Press.
Hammond, K. (2004). Monsters of modernity: Frankenstein and modern environmentalism. Cultural Geographies, 11(2), 181–198.
Harrington, B. (2008). Monsters of the complex market. https://thesocietypages.org/economicsociology/tag/frankenstein/. Accessed July 26, 2016.
Hart-Brinson, P. (2016). The social imagination of homosexuality and the rise of same-sex marriage in the United States. Socius: Sociological Research for a Dynamic World. doi:10.1177/2378023116630555.
Haynes, R. (1995). Frankenstein: The scientist we love to hate. Public Understanding of Science, 4(4), 435–444.
Haynes, R. (2003). From alchemy to artificial intelligence: Stereotypes of the scientist in Western literature. Public Understanding of Science, 12(3), 243–253.
Haynes, R. D. (2016). Whatever happened to the ‘mad, bad’ scientist? Overturning the stereotype. Public Understanding of Science, 25(1), 31–44.
Heffernan, T. (2003). Bovine anxieties, virgin births, and the secret of life. Cultural Critique, 53, 116–133.
Hellsten, I. (2003). Focus on metaphors: The case of “Frankenfood” on the Web. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 8(4), 1–15.
Herek, G. M. (2007). Confronting sexual stigma and prejudice: Theory and practice. Journal of Social Issues, 63(4), 905–925.
Hindle, M. (1990). Vital matters: Mary Shelley’s Frankenstein and Romantic science. Critical Survey, 2(1), 29–35.
Jacobson, N., & Greenley, D. (2001). What is recovery? A conceptual model and explication. Psychiatric Services, 52(4), 482–485.
Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system-justification and the production of false consciousness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33(1), 1–27.
Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system justification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the Status quo. Political Psychology, 25(6), 881–919.
Jost, J. T., Brett, P. W., Sheldon, O., & Sullivan, B. N. (2003). Social inequality and the reduction of ideological dissonance on behalf of the system: Evidence of enhanced system justification among the disadvantaged. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33(1), 13–36.
Jost, J. T., Liviatan, I., van der Toorn, J., Ledgerwood, A., Mandisodza, A., & Nosek, B. A. (2010). System justification: How do we know it’s motivated? In D. R. Bobocel, A. C. Kay, M. P. Zanna, & J. M. Olson (Eds.), The psychology of justice and legitimacy: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 11, pp. 173–204). London: Taylor & Francis.
Kahan, D. M., Jenkins-Smith, H., & Braman, D. (2010). Cultural cognition of scientific consensus. Journal of Risk Research, 14(2), 147–174.
Kahan, D. M., Peters, E., Wittlin, M., Slovic, P., Ouellette, L. L., Braman, D., et al. (2012). The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate change risks. Nature Climate Change, 2, 732–735.
Kaiser, C. R. (2006). Dominant ideology threat and the interpersonal consequences of attributions to discrimination. In S. Levin & C. van Laar (Eds.), Stigma and group inequality: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 45–64). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Kirkwood, A. D., & Stamm, B. H. (2006). A social marketing approach to challenging stigma. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 37(5), 472–476.
Kitzinger, J. (2010). Questioning the sci-fi alibi: A critique of how ‘science fiction fears’ are used to explain away public concerns about risk. Journal of Risk Research, 13(1), 73–86.
Knust, H. (1983). From Faust to Oppenheimer: The scientist’s pact with the Devil. Journal of European Studies, 13(49–50), 122–141.
Lewandowsky, S., Ecker, U. K. H., Seifert, C. M., Schwarz, N., & Cook, J. (2012). Misinformation and its correction: Continued influence and successful debiasing. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 13(3), 106–131.
Liakopoulos, M. (2002). Pandora’s Box or panacea? Using metaphors to create the public representations of biotechnology. Public Understanding of Science, 11(1), 5–32.
Link, H. S. (2013). Playing God and the intrinsic value of life: Moral problems for synthetic biology? Science and Engineering Ethics, 19(2), 435–448.
Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. C. (2001). Conceptualizing stigma. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 363–385.
Locke, S. (1999). Golem science and the public understanding of science: From deficit to dilemma. Public Understanding of Science, 8(2), 75–92.
Losh, S. C. (2010). Stereotypes about scientists over time among US adults: 1983 and 2001. Public Understanding of Science, 19(3), 372–382.
Lupia, A. (2013). Communicating science in politicized environments. PNAS, 110(3), 14048–14054.
Major, B., & O’Brien, L. T. (2005). The social psychology of stigma. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 393–421.
Marcus, S. (2002). Frankenstein: Myths of scientific and medical knowledge and stories of human relations. The Southern Review, 38(1), 188–201.
Marsh, E. J., & Fazio, L. K. (2006). Learning errors from fiction: Difficulties in reducing reliance on fictional stories. Memory & Cognition, 34(5), 1140–1149.
Marsh, E. J., Meade, M. L., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2003). Learning facts from fiction. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(4), 519–536.
Mazlish, B. (1995). The man–machine and artificial intelligence. Stanford Humanities Review, 4(2), 21–45.
Meisenbach, R. J. (2010). Stigma management communication: A theory and agenda for applied research on how individuals manage moments of stigmatized identity. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 38(3), 268–292.
Mulkay, M. (1996). Frankenstein and the debate over embryo research. Science, Technology and Human Values, 21(2), 157–176.
Nadelson, L., Jorcyk, C., Yang, D., Smith, M. J., Matson, S., Cornell, K., et al. (2014). I just don’t trust them: The development and validation of an assessment instrument to measure trust in science and scientists. School Science and Mathematics, 114(2), 76–86.
Newman, W. R. (2004). Promethean ambitions: Alchemy and the quest to perfect nature. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Nisbet, M. (2009). Framing science: A new paradigm in public engagement. In L. Kahlor & P. A. Stout (Eds.), Communicating science: New agendas in communication (pp. 40–67). New York: Routledge.
Oyserman, D., & Swim, J. K. (2001). Stigma: An insider’s view. Journal of Social Issues, 57(1), 1–14.
Pachankis, J. E. (2007). The Psychological implication of concealing a stigma: A cognitive–affective–behavioral model. Psychological Bulletin, 133(2), 328–345.
Parrington, J. (2016). Redesigning life: How genome editing will transform the world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Peláez, A. L., & Kyriakou, D. (2008). Robots, genes and bytes: Technology development and social changes towards the year 2020. Technological Forecasting and Social Change, 75(8), 1176–1201.
Pescosolido, B. A., Martin, J. K., Lang, A., & Olafsdottir, S. (2008). Rethinking theoretical approaches to stigma: A framework integrating normative influences on stigma (FINIS). Social Science and Medicine, 67(3), 431–440.
PETA UK. (2016). Creating human-animal hybrids is bad for people—And worse for animals. http://www.peta.org.uk/blog/creating-human-animal-hybrids-bad-people-worse-animals/. Accessed January 11, 2017.
Petersen, A. (2002). Replicating our bodies, losing our selves: News media portrayals of human cloning in the wake of Dolly. Body & Society, 8(4), 71–90.
Pryor, J. B., Reeder, G. D., Yeadon, C., & Hesson-McInnis, M. (2004). A dual-process model of reactions to perceived stigma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(4), 436–452.
Reed, R. (2001). (Un-)Professional discourse? Journalists’ and scientists’ stories about science in the media. Journalism, 2(3), 279–298.
Reis, P., & Galvão, C. (2004). Socio-scientific controversies and students’ conceptions about scientists. International Journal of Science Education, 26(13), 1621–1633.
Roese, N. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Hindsight Bias. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 411–426.
Rutjens, B. T., & Heine, S. J. (2016). The immoral landscape? Scientists are associated with violations of morality. PLoS ONE, 11(4), e0152798.
Shih, M. (2004). Positive stigma: Examining resilience and empowerment in overcoming stigma. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 591(1), 175–185.
Skal, D. J. (1998). Screams of reason: Mad science and modern culture. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.
Speaking of Research. (2016). Why we haven’t cured the common cold—A response to PETA’s science advisor, Dr. Julia Baines. https://speakingofresearch.com/tag/peta/. Accessed January 12, 2017.
Stableford, B. (1995). Frankenstein and the origins of science fiction. In D. Seed (Ed.), Anticipations: Essays on early science fiction and its precursors (pp. 46–57). Syracuse: Syracuse University Press.
Stein, Y. (2005). The psychoanalysis of science: The role of metaphor, paraprax, lacunae and myth. Portland: Sussex Academic Press.
Swart, S. (2014). Frankenzebra: Dangerous knowledge and the narrative of the construction of monsters. Journal of Literary Studies, 30(4), 45–70.
Syrdal, D. S., Nomura, T., Hirai, H., & Dautenhahn, K. (2011). Examining the Frankenstein syndrome: An open-ended cross-cultural survey (pp. 125–134). In B. Mutlu, C. Bartneck, J. Ham, V. Evers & T. Kanda (Eds.), Social robotics: Third international conference, ICSR 2011, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, November 24–25, 2011. Proceedings. Berlin: Springer.
Tan, A. L., Jocz, J. A., & Zhai, J. (2015). Spiderman and science: How students’ perceptions of scientists are shaped by popular media. Public Understanding of Science. doi:10.1177/0963662515615086.
The National Academies of Sciences. (2017). Communicating science effectively: A research agenda. Washington: The National Academies Press.
Turney, J. (1998). Frankenstein’s footsteps: Science, genetics and popular culture. New Haven: Yale University Press.
van der Laan, J. M. (2010). Frankenstein as science fiction and fact. Bulletin of Science, Technology & Society, 30(4), 298–304.
van Dijck, J. (1999). Cloning humans, cloning literature: Genetics and the imagination deficit. New Genetics & Society, 18(1), 9–22.
van Dyke, N., & Cress, R. (2006). Political opportunities and collective identity in Ohio’s gay and lesbian movement, 1970–2000. Sociological Perspectives, 49(4), 503–526.
Webber, A. J. (2003). The Doppelgänger: Double visions in German literature. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Weingart, P., Muhl, C., & Pansegrau, P. (2003). Of power maniacs and unethical geniuses: Science and scientists in fiction film. Public Understanding of Science, 12(3), 279–287.
Whitson, J. A., Galinsky, A. D., & Kay, A. (2015). The emotional roots of conspiratorial perceptions, system justification, and belief in the paranormal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 89–95.
Wurzman, R., Hamilton, R. H., Pascual-Leone, A., & Fox, M. D. (2016). An open letter concerning do-it-yourself users of transcranial direct current stimulation. Annals of Neurology, 80(1), 1–4.
Zhu, L. L., Kay, A. C., & Eibach, R. P. (2013). A test of the flexible ideology hypothesis: System justification motives interact with ideological cueing to predict political judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 755–758.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Phil Weaver-Stoesz, the editors and the anonymous reviewers at Science and Engineering Ethics for their guidance and thoughtful comments regarding our work. We also would like to gratefully acknowledge the support of the Center for Science and the Imagination; and King Coffee in Tempe, Arizona.
Funding
This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. 1516684.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Nagy, P., Wylie, R., Eschrich, J. et al. Why Frankenstein is a Stigma Among Scientists. Sci Eng Ethics 24, 1143–1159 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11948-017-9936-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11948-017-9936-9