Abstract
The primate prefrontal cortex (PFC) subserves our highest order cognitive operations, and yet is tremendously dependent on a precise neurochemical environment for proper functioning. Depletion of noradrenaline and dopamine, or of acetylcholine from the dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC), is as devastating as removing the cortex itself, and serotonergic influences are also critical to proper functioning of the orbital and medial PFC. Most neuromodulators have a narrow inverted U dose response, which coordinates arousal state with cognitive state, and contributes to cognitive deficits with fatigue or uncontrollable stress. Studies in monkeys have revealed the molecular signaling mechanisms that govern the generation and modulation of mental representations by the dlPFC, allowing dynamic regulation of network strength, a process that requires tight regulation to prevent toxic actions, e.g., as occurs with advanced age. Brain imaging studies in humans have observed drug and genotype influences on a range of cognitive tasks and on PFC circuit functional connectivity, e.g., showing that catecholamines stabilize representations in a baseline-dependent manner. Research in monkeys has already led to new treatments for cognitive disorders in humans, encouraging future research in this important field.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The primate prefrontal cortex (PFC) has the extraordinary ability to represent information in the absence of sensory stimulation, the foundation of abstract thought. The PFC generates our mental arena, and subserves our highest order functions, such as abstract reasoning, working memory, high-order decision making, planning, and organization, providing top-down control of attention, actions, and emotions. However, the PFC is remarkably sensitive to its neurochemical environment, where either depletion or overstimulation by the neuromodulators—acetylcholine, catecholamines, and serotonin—impairs its function. Neuromodulators are typically released in PFC in response to salient events, and act over a long time scale (e.g., on the order of seconds) by way of G-coupled proteins to alter the impact of nearby neurotransmission. Indeed, in the dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC), glutamate neurotransmission requires acetylcholine actions, emphasizing the importance of neuromodulation to PFC function. These powerful influences by neuromodulators coordinate our cognitive and emotional states with environmental events and our state of arousal, which can have survival value, e.g., saving energy when fatigued, or rapidly switching control of behavior to more reflexive circuits in response to danger. However, these built-in mechanisms to take the PFC “off-line” also render the PFC especially vulnerable to dysfunction. This can be particularly problematic when these signaling events are not properly regulated, e.g., due to genetic mutations and/or inflammatory insults, rendering the PFC susceptible to atrophy and degeneration. Thus, understanding neuromodulatory influences on PFC circuits can help to explain why these circuits are so often impaired in mental disorders.
Importantly, these modulatory actions appear to vary based on PFC subregion, circuit, cell-type, and receptor, with no universal actions. There are also large species differences between rodents and primates [1, 2], adding further complexity. Given these species differences, and the great expansion and differentiation of the PFC in primates [3, 4], this review will focus on neuromodulation of the human and nonhuman primate PFC by the arousal systems. The most detailed mechanistic information has focused on layer III of the dlPFC, as this layer contains the microcircuits that generate mental representations needed for working memory. As these neurons are greatly affected in schizophrenia [5, 6] and Alzheimer’s Disease [7], understanding their modulation has had particular clinical relevance, and will be a focus in this review. However, it is likely that other PFC circuits have very different molecular needs, which may relate to their specialized functions. Studies of PFC neuromodulation in humans are also reviewed, as these provided a complementary, systems-wide view, with immediate relevance to our understanding of how cognitive impairments can arise when there are disruptions in neuromodulatory mechanisms.
Please note that there are large arenas where little or no research has been performed in this field, and these discrepancies are reflected in the degree of discussion in this review. For example, there has been little research on the effects of serotonin at the cellular/molecular level in primate dlPFC, and thus this section is necessarily brief. Similarly, selective dopamine (DA) D1 receptor (D1R) agonists have been under development but have yet to be approved for widespread human use, and thus the section on neuromodulation of PFC functions in humans reflects the more frequent studies using D2R or mixed DA receptor agonists. The reader is also cautioned that methylphenidate, which is often referred to as a DAergic compound, actually increases NA as well as DA availability in the PFC [8, 9]. Finally, the reader is reminded that PFC functions are carried out as part of larger networks, e.g., involving connections with posterior cortex and subcortical structures, including striatal-cortical circuits. A review of these connections is beyond the scope of the current article, but can be found elsewhere in this special volume (e.g., Haber and Bullmore). By analogy, the putative differential consequences of neuromodulation across different brain regions are also beyond the scope of this review. Clearly neuromodulatory actions in these other nodes also contribute to overall network activity and cognitive and behavioral change.
A brief review of PFC topography
The large, highly differentiated primate PFC is topographically organized along multiple dimensions, with more newly evolved areas situated more rostrally, and more primitive areas and functions situated more caudally [3]. Rostral and dlPFC subregions expand from marmoset to rhesus macaque to human [10, 11] suggesting that some aspects of dlPFC functions may be better studied in macaques than in New World monkeys. Imaging studies in humans have shown that more rostral PFC areas are activated by more abstract representations and manipulations (e.g., metacognition), while more caudal PFC areas are activated by simpler representations [12, 13]. Although it is challenging to study metacognition in macaques, data show that the frontal pole is also important for self-evaluation in monkeys [14]. Anatomists have long recognized that inputs to the PFC are also organized along a dorsolateral to ventromedial spectrum, with the dorsal and lateral PFC areas receiving projections from the visual and auditory association cortices, thus representing the external world, while the ventral and medial PFC areas receive inputs about stimuli impinging on or within our bodies (taste, smell, somatosensation including pain and viscera) and represent our internal world [15, 16]. For example, circuits in the dlPFC subserve visuospatial working memory, and are able to maintain a representation of visual space without sensory stimulation for many seconds. These dlPFC neurons can maintain representations even in the face of distractors, a property that may be unique to PFC [17], with projections back to the association cortices, thalamus, and basal ganglia to mediate top-down regulation of attention [18]. The dlPFC can also influence motor responding through projections back to the frontal eye fields, premotor areas and basal ganglia, including the ability to inhibit inappropriate responses, e.g., [19]. In contrast, neurons in the ventral PFC (often called orbital PFC) generate flexible representations of reward values to guide decision making, e.g., the changing reward value of chocolate or other palatable foods as one eats to satiation [20, 21]. These areas can mediate our emotional responses via connections to the medial PFC, specifically the anterior cingulate cortex (BA24) and subgenual cingulate cortex (BA25), which have outputs to hypothalamus, ventral striatum, amygdala, and brainstem to regulate our emotional responses, including autonomic regulation of the viscera [16]. The rostral areas of PFC and the dlPFC can also contribute to “top-down” emotional regulation via the ventromedial PFC (vmPFC), e.g., dlPFC area 46 connects to BA10m and BA32, and BA32 in turn projects to BA24 and BA25 to regulate emotional response [22,23,24].
It is also important to note that neural activity is distributed throughout cortical networks, e.g., reflected in neuronal firing and BOLD responses throughout cortex. However, lesion studies in monkeys have been particularly helpful in defining the essential and distinctive contributions of nodes in a network, e.g., the dlPFC is required for the generation of persistent firing needed for working memory and selective attention, which is then reflected in more posterior cortical areas [17, 25]; Panichello, 2021 #8302}. Thus, neurochemical actions in a single node may have ramifications for activity patterns throughout the brain.
We will see below that different PFC subregions have differing neurochemical dependencies. For example, depletion of serotonin greatly impairs the affective regulation by the orbital PFC, but has little effect on the visuospatial working memory functions of the dlPFC [26,27,28,29]. However, in-depth studies of neuromodulatory actions at the cellular level are rare in primates, and thus the reader is cautioned that there is still much to be learned in this important scientific arena.
The arousal systems—the activity patterns of monoamine and cholinergic neurons
The neurons that synthesize monoamines or acetylcholine reside in the brainstem and basal forebrain (Fig. 1; see list below), with axonal projections throughout much of brain.
Although previous research suggested that many of these neurons projected throughout the brain and cortex, more recent research from rodents suggests there may be much greater heterogeneity than previously suspected, e.g., with a subgroup of locus coeruleus neurons selectively targeting the PFC and not the motor cortex [35]. These monoamine and cholinergic nuclei are part of the arousal systems that control sleep vs. waking states [36]. However, most pertinent to the current review, these modulators also orchestrate brain activity within the waking state, coordinating and fine-tuning brain activity in response to external and internal events.
There are regional and laminar differences in the density and pattern of innervation in the primate PFC [37,38,39]. For example, DA innervation of the vmPFC is denser than in dlPFC [40, 41]. However, lower levels of innervation do not signify that an input is less important: the catecholamine innervation of dlPFC is quite modest, but selective lesioning of these axons is as devastating as removing the cortex itself [26].
PFC subregions also project back down to the cell bodies of the arousal systems to regulate brain state. The vmPFC in macaque (BA32 and BA25) projects to the dorsal raphe (5HT and DA), SNc, VTA (DA), and the LC (NE) [42] (note: only BA25 projects to the LC [42]). The orbital PFC projects to the cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain [43], while the dlPFC projects to the LC, raphe, and midbrain DA neurons [30, 44]. Neuromodulatory neurons also receive inputs from subcortical structures, which may provide more primitive regulation. For example, the cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain not only receive inputs form the orbital, entorhinal, and insular cortices, but also from the nucleus accumbens and hypothalamus [43]. Similarly, the NA neurons in the locus coeruleus not only receive inputs from PFC [30, 42], but from the amygdala [45], which in rodents activates the LC during stress [46]. Thus, the PFC is positioned to regulate its own neuromodulatory inputs, with the dlPFC projecting to the monoamine nuclei, and the orbital PFC projecting to the cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain.
In general, all of these neuromodulatory arousal systems have increased firing during waking and reduced firing during deep sleep. However, there are differences related to stage of sleep, e.g., where noradrenergic LC neurons shut off completely during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, while cholinergic neurons are activated during REM sleep (see [36] for review). Within the waking state, modulatory neurons that project to the PFC generally show increased firing to salient events. This has been demonstrated for noradrenergic neurons in the primate LC and Ach neurons in the basal forebrain, which fire to salient events, whether they are of positive or negative valence, with differing modes of baseline firing based on levels of arousal (e.g., calm vs. agitated) [47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54]. These alterations in firing, and coincident levels of release, are thought to optimize attention, e.g., by orienting attention to a novel stimulus [55], and by coordinating brain state for appropriate control of visceral and motor responses. The LC also shows enhanced firing to energize behavior for an effortful task [56]. As described below, the degree of LC firing and thus of NA release would differentially engage adrenergic receptor subtypes based on their affinity for NA, where high affinity (α2A) receptors would be engaged with moderate levels of release, and low affinity receptors (α1A, β1) with high levels of release, enhancing vs. impairing PFC function. It is possible that other modulatory systems act in this way as well. Recordings from the dorsal raphe in monkeys find phasic increases in firing to rewarding stimuli, and prolonged responses to aversive stimuli [57]. Although little is known about how serotonin alters PFC operations at its many receptors, there may be parallel actions at high affinity (e.g., 5-HT1A) vs. lower affinity (e.g., 5-HT2) receptors [58] in cortex based on the degree and pattern of raphe neuronal firing.
Recordings from DA neurons in the primate have shown discreet subpopulations with differing responses to rewarding vs. aversive events: there are “Value” neurons that increase their firing to events associated with reward, and decrease their firing to loss of reward [59]. However, there are also “Salience” DA neurons that increase their firing to both rewarding and aversive events [60, 61]. The DA neurons projecting to dlPFC appear to be “Salience” DA neurons, as DA levels increase rather than decrease in response to loss of reward [62]. It is not known if the DA neurons that project to medial PFC in primates also fire to salient events as has been shown for DA projections to medial PFC in rodent [63]. It is also possible that there is a mixture of DA “Salience” and DA “Value” neurons projecting to the primate PFC, with differing proportions in different subregions. Better understanding of this issue would require biochemical surveys of the PFC in response to gain and loss of rewards. However, fMRI studies in macaque indicate that activity in the dorsal raphe reflects changes in the global reward state [64], consistent with serotonergic neurons in mice responding to salient events [65].
Effects of monoamine or cholinergic depletion on dlPFC vs. orbital PFC function
Neurotoxic lesions of these neuromodulatory systems have large effects on PFC functioning, although there can be qualitatively different effects based on neuromodulator and PFC subregion [66]. Extensive depletion of the catecholamines, or of Ach, but not of serotonin, markedly impairs the working memory and attention functions of the dlPFC in rhesus monkeys and marmosets [26, 27, 67, 68], including impairment of attentional set-shifting and increased distractibility [67]. We will see below that Ach plays a key permissive role in NMDAR neurotransmission in dlPFC, and that catecholamines have a powerful effect on synaptic connectivity, consistent with these depletion data. In contrast to the catecholamines and acetylcholine, serotonin depletion from the dlPFC in marmosets has little effect on performance of an attentional set-shifting task [27]. However, serotonin depletion from the ventral PFC has a very large effect [29], e.g., impairing reversal learning by failing to inhibit responding to the previously rewarded stimulus [69]. DA depletion also altered ventral PFC function, but with a qualitatively different profile. Thus, marmosets with serotonin depletion of the ventral PFC displayed stimulus-bound responding, consistent with a role in preventing competing, task-irrelevant, salient stimuli from biasing responding [28]. In contrast, monkeys with DA depletion displayed a pattern of deficits consistent with basic deficits in the associative processing of reward [28]. Reversal learning was also impaired by lesioning of the cholinergic nbM in marmosets [70, 71], suggesting that both monoamines and acetylcholine influence ventral PFC function. Overall, these data suggest that the ventral and dlPFC share a reliance on cholinergic and catecholamine actions, with the exception that the ventral PFC appears more dependent on serotonin. However, it is likely that more refined serotonergic manipulations, e.g., selective blockade of 5HT2A receptor actions [72], may reveal serotonergic contributions to dlPFC function that are not evident with global depletion. Most importantly, these studies have shown that depletion of neuromodulators can be as devastating as removing the PFC itself [26, 68], emphasizing the tremendous reliance of PFC circuits on their neuromodulatory environment.
In-depth analyses of neuromodulation of rhesus monkey dlPFC
The primate dlPFC is essential for working memory, and contains “Delay cells” that maintain mental representations without sensory stimulation. This fundamental operation contributes to many dlPFC functions including working memory [73], cognitive control [74], and attention regulation [17, 75]. Delay cells are able to generate and maintain persistent firing for many seconds (e.g., >15 s in Fuster’s original studies [76, 77]), representing a visual feature, a rule, or as shown in Fig. 2A, a position in visual space [73]. Strong working memory requires both persistent firing and spatial tuning such that specific information can be held in working memory stores. The cellular basis underlying Delay cell firing was discovered by Goldman-Rakic et al. in vivo [78] and confirmed by Gonzalez-Burgos et al. in vitro [79, 80]. They found microcircuits in deep layer III with extensive horizontal connections, where recurrent excitation between pyramidal cells with shared characteristic generate the persistent neuronal firing needed to maintain mental representations in working memory (Fig. 2C). They also found that these microcircuits contain fast-spiking, parvalbumin-containing GABAergic interneurons that are activated by lateral inhibition to refine the contents of working memory (Fig. 2C). The roles of additional types of interneurons in these microcircuits are still under investigation, e.g., where calretinin-containing interneurons are enriched in dlPFC and may inhibit other interneurons to boost pyramidal cell persistent firing [81]. It should also be noted that the persistent firing during a working memory task is reflected in local field potentials as increased gamma band, and reduced beta band, oscillations [82]. As local field potentials capture neuronal activity over a wide area of cortex, they are able to detect the movement of information throughout a network when the task becomes complicated by additional challenges [83].
As described in the next sections, Delay cell firing arising from recurrent excitation in deep layer III dlPFC depends on both unusual neurotransmission and neuromodulation, where the arousal systems play a critical role in permitting and shaping network firing.
Unique neurotransmission in dlPFC
Neurons in classic circuits, such as primate primary visual cortex (V1), rely on AMPAR neurotransmission, including AMPAR permissive actions for NMDAR neurotransmission. In contrast, dlPFC Delay cells have surprisingly little reliance on AMPAR, and instead depend on NMDAR with permissive actions from acetylcholine (Fig. 2D).
In primate V1, blockade of AMPAR markedly reduces stimulus-evoked neuronal firing [84], and AMPAR are needed to process feedforward sensory input from thalamus [85]. NMDARs contribute less to processing of visual stimuli [84], although they contribute to top-down feedback regulation and local recurrence [85]. In classic circuits, AMPAR play an essential permissive role for NMDAR actions, depolarizing the postsynaptic membrane to eject the Mg2+ block from the NMDAR ion channel pore [86]. The rapid kinetics of AMPARs are appropriate for these circuits, where timing onset and offset are important for encoding a sensory event [87]. NMDAR with GluN2B subunits, which close slowly and flux high levels of calcium, are often extrasynaptic in classic circuits, where they contribute to excitotoxicity [88, 89].
In contrast to classic circuits, Delay cell circuits in the dlPFC have a very different function than neurons in V1: they must generate and sustain mental representations without sensory stimulation. This operation relies heavily on recurrent excitatory circuits mediated by NMDAR [90] (Fig. 2C, D), a finding predicted by computational models [91]. Thus, even low-dose blockade of NMDAR, including antagonists that selectively block those with NMDAR- GluN2A or NMDAR- GluN2B subunits, markedly reduces Delay cell firing (Fig. 2B; [91]). NMDAR- GluN2B are found exclusively within the postsynaptic density (PSD) in layer III dlPFC, consistent with their direct mediation of neurotransmission [91]. The large calcium flux by GluN2B subunits may be a key aspect of why they are needed to support persistent firing in computational models [91] and neurons [90].
In contrast to their great reliance on NMDAR, Delay cells show surprisingly subtle changes when AMPAR are blocked (Fig. 2D; [90]). This finding was unexpected, given that in classic circuits, AMPAR are essential to depolarize the PSD membrane and relieve the Mg2+ block within the NMDAR pore. What mechanism supplied this vital function for Delay cells in dlPFC?
The critical role of acetylcholine for NMDAR neurotransmission in dlPFC
In dlPFC, the key permissive role for NMDAR neurotransmission appears to be played by acetylcholine acting at Nic-α7R and muscarinic M1R within the glutamate synapse [92, 93], which may depolarize the PSD to support persistent firing (Fig. 2D). Nic-α7R are ion channels that can directly depolarize the postsynaptic membrane, and can be found in glutamatergic synapses in primate dlPFC [92]. M1R also contribute and may depolarize the postsynaptic membrane indirectly [93], e.g., by closing neighboring KCNQ K+ channels that are also found within the glutamatergic PSD (schematized in Fig. 2B), and/or by increasing intracellular Ca2+ release near the synapse via IP3 signaling (Fig. 3A). These data are consistent with behavioral data showing that Ach depletion from dlPFC is as deleterious as removing the cortex itself [68]. As acetylcholine is released during wakefulness but not during deep sleep [36], these mechanisms also help to coordinate cognitive state with arousal state, permitting conscious experience during wakefulness, but may render us unconscious during deep sleep when there is no acetylcholine release.
It should be noted that high levels of muscarinic M1R signaling reduce Delay cell firing, inducing an inverted U dose/response [93,94,95,96]. This may involve M1R excitation of GABAergic interneurons, and/or excessive IP3-mediated calcium-cAMP signaling opening of K+ channels (Fig. 3A). These inverted U dose-response curves have also seen at the behavioral level, where systemic administration of selective M1R agonists or PAMs improve working memory at low doses, but lose efficacy and/or impair cognition at higher doses [93]. These inverted U dose-response curves can make therapeutic translation challenging, and yet are the rule, rather than exception, in PFC, including for catecholamine neuromodulation, as described in the next section.
Unique neuromodulation in dlPFC
In classic circuits, calcium-cAMP-PKA signaling strengthens circuits, e.g., by enhancing transmitter release and/or promoting longer-term plasticity, while for dlPFC Delay cells, high levels of cAMP signaling weaken connectivity by opening potassium (K+) channels on spines (Fig. 3). These differences are emphasized by comparing cAMP actions in primate V1 to dlPFC. In primate V1, the cAMP-related protein, PDE4A, is localized in glutamatergic presynaptic terminals surrounding synaptic vesicle, and increasing cAMP-PKA signaling produces a linear increase in neuronal firing to visual stimuli [84]. This contrasts with the dlPFC, where cAMP-PKA signaling proteins are concentrated post-synaptically in spines. As shown in Fig. 3A, there is extensive molecular machinery for cAMP-PKA signaling to magnify calcium actions in spines. cAMP-PKA phosphorylation of NMDAR is needed to maintain NMDAR in the PSD and enhances calcium flux through NMDAR [97,98,99]. In addition, immunoEM has revealed a large constellation of cAMP signaling proteins on the calcium storing smooth endoplasmic reticulum (SER), known as the spine apparatus where it elaborates inside of spines [100]. cAMP-PKA signaling can increase calcium release through ryanodine receptors (RyR) and IP3 receptors (IP3R) on the SER. Calcium release into the cytosol can in turn activate adenylyl cyclase to generate cAMP, producing feedforward signaling [100]. Moderate levels of calcium-cAMP signaling are needed for Delay cell firing, likely by helping to maintain a depolarized PSD to support persistent firing (Fig. 3A). However, these spines also contain K+ channels (HCN, KCNQ) whose open state is increased by cAMP or PKA, needed for negative feedback and to allow dynamic shaping of network connectivity [101]. Importantly, high levels of cAMP-K+ channel opening markedly decrease Delay cell firing (Fig. 3A). These signaling events contribute to the narrow, inverted U dose response in Delay cell firing, as described below for both DA and NE.
Given the power of feedforward signaling events to reduce network connectivity, it is particularly important that calcium-cAMP signaling is tightly regulated (Fig. 3B). The regulation of cAMP production plays a powerful role in strengthening dlPFC network connectivity, persistent firing and working memory function. For example, metabotropic glutamate receptors mGluR3, which are primarily presynaptic in rodent, have a large postsynaptic role in primate layer III dlPFC, where they inhibit cAMP-K+ signaling and enhance working memory-related firing [102]. mGluR3 are stimulated not only by glutamate, but by NAAG, which is co-released with glutamate and is selective for mGluR3. NAAG is catabolized by GCPII, which is increased by inflammation [103]. These signaling mechanisms appear to have direct relevance to human cognition, as genetic insults to mGluR3 (GRM3) are a risk factor for schizophrenia [104], and a genetic alteration that increases GCPII production and lowers mGluR3 signaling is associated with inefficient dlPFC activity and lower IQ [105, 106]. Thus, the evolutionary expansion of postsynaptic mGluR3 signaling in PFC appears to be particularly important to human intelligence. A similar mechanism is described below, where NA stimulation of α2A-AR on spines inhibits cAMP-K+ channel signaling and enhances Delay cell firing and PFC cognitive functions.
Feedforward calcium-cAMP-K+ channels signaling is also regulated by the PDE4s that catabolize cAMP [107,108,109], and by calbindin, which binds calcium in the cytosol [110]. PDE4 inhibition reduces Delay cell firing [107, 109], very different from the enhancing effects on plasticity in hippocampus [111]. PDE4s are anchored to the spine apparatus by DISC1 (Disrupted In SChizophrenia) [108], which is weakened with inflammation [112, 113], or by genetic insults in a large British family with high rates of mental illness [114]. As summarized in Fig. 3C, loss of regulation by inflammation, advancing age, and/or genetic insults weakens connectivity, reduces neuronal firing, and impairs cognitive functions dependent upon the dlPFC. For example, there is evidence of reduced PDE4, calbindin, mGluR3, and α2A-AR expression with advancing age in the rhesus monkey and/or human dlPFC [109, 110, 115, 116] and a loss of Delay cell firing in aged monkeys induced by excessive calcium-cAMP-K+ channel signaling [117]. Dysregulated calcium-cAMP signaling is also associated with increased phosphorylation of tau [109, 110, 118]. Thus, regulation of these signaling events is key to cognitive health.
The benefits of magnified calcium-cAMP signaling near the NMDAR synapse, vs. the increased opening of nearby K+ channels, help to explain the inverted U dose-response curve between arousal modulators such as DA and NA and PFC cognitive functioning, as described in the following sections.
Dopamine neuromodulation in dlPFC
The first evidence that DA was essential to dlPFC working memory function was surprisingly early, when in 1979 Patricia Goldman (not yet Goldman-Rakic) and colleagues published their landmark study showing that depletion of DA from the primate dlPFC produced as profound a deficit as removing the cortex itself [26]. Although the effective lesion depleted both DA and NE, and we now know that both are critical to dlPFC function, Goldman-Rakic’s later work showed that D1R antagonist infusions into the dlPFC also impaired working memory [119], supporting her earlier hypothesis, and opening an entire arena of research.
DA acts at a variety of receptors, but most research works on its actions in dlPFC have focused on D1R, which are the most prominent subtype in this cortex [120], and which generally couple through Gs to increase cAMP production. In monkey and human layer III dlPFC, D1R are predominately localized on pyramidal cell spines, with a much smaller proportion expressed by parvalbumin-containing interneurons [41, 108, 121, 122]. D1R have an inverted U dose-response relationship on working memory capabilities [123,124,125,126], and on Delay cell firing [41, 122, 127, 128], e.g., as illustrated in Fig. 4. Thus, either too little DA D1R stimulation, (e.g., as occurs with high-dose D1R blockade [127] or aging [122]), or excessive D1R stimulation (as occurs with stress [124, 129] or high doses of D1R agonist [122, 128]), reduces Delay cell firing and impairs working memory. An optimal dose of D1R agonist suppressed “noise” while maintaining neuronal firing for a neuron’s preferred direction, narrowing the contents of working memory [128]. Similar inverted U effects have been seen for D1R modulation of dynamic dlPFC encoding of rules underlying cognitive control [130, 131], suggesting that these actions may generalize to dlPFC representational capabilities independent of the specific operation. As described below, the inverted U DA D1R dose response on working memory and cognitive control can also be seen in humans [132], including the narrowing of representations [133, 134], suggesting this aspect translates across species. The “narrowing” of representations by higher levels of D1R stimulation may also contribute to the increased stability, but reduced updating, of representations seen in human subjects given methylphenidate [135] (see discussion of DA effects in humans, below).
The beneficial, excitatory effects of D1R stimulation have been challenging to capture until recently, as traditional D1R agonists have had very high affinity for the receptor, whereas DA itself has relatively low affinity [122]. Thus, high affinity agonists could mimic high levels of receptor stimulation, e.g., as occurs with stress, but could not mimic the more subtle actions that occur under nonstress conditions. The reader should be cautioned that most of the research described in this section was performed with high affinity D1R agonists that do not mimic the effects of endogenous DA, although they appear to capture the effects of high levels of endogenous DA release, e.g., as occurs with stress exposure. The recent creation of low affinity D1R agonists that better mimic DA have allowed the first, direct evidence of DA D1R excitatory actions on Delay cell firing [122], e.g., as seen in Fig. 4B. D1R have been shown to enhance NMDAR actions in rodent PFC, e.g., via phosphorylation of NMDAR to maintain them in the PSD [97, 98] and to enhance calcium flux [99]. It is likely that these actions also occur in primate layer III dlPFC, where PKA-phosphorylated NMDAR can be seen in the PSD using immunoEM [90]. These actions may contribute to the beneficial, excitatory effects of D1R stimulation (schematically illustrated in Fig. 4A, B).
With increasing levels of D1R stimulation, cAMP signaling opens K+ channels to gate out network inputs, preferentially reducing the neuron’s response to nonpreferred directions (“noise”), while retaining delay-related firing to preferred inputs (“signal”) [128]. This narrowing of the mental representation may be helpful in refining representations in the face of interference. However, further increases in D1R stimulation, e.g., as occurs with uncontrollable stress, reduces neuronal firing to signals as well as noise, eroding rather than refining neural representations. High levels of D1R stimulation, even with the lower affinity agonist, cause a marked loss of Delay cell firing (Fig. 4C) [122, 128] and working memory deficits with either local or systemic D1R agonist administration [41, 125, 126, 131]. High levels of D1R stimulation also render pyramidal cells more vulnerable to distractors [136]. Conversely, a D1R antagonist can reverse the effects of uncontrollable stress on working memory performance [124].
The intracellular signaling events that mediate the detrimental effects of high-dose D1R agonists on Delay cell firing and working memory are illustrated in Fig. 4C; the loss of dlPFC Delay cell firing can be blocked by a PKA inhibitor [128] or an HCN channel antagonist [137]. Thus, the ability of D1R to strengthen vs. weaken synaptic activity may be related to both the degree of cAMP-calcium signaling, and the location(s) of D1R on the spine membrane on a particular network connection on a spine, where D1R within/near the PSD may be more likely to strengthen firing, while those at more distant locations from the PSD but close to HCN and KCNQ channels on the spine may promote the gating of network inputs [41], and with higher levels (e.g., during uncontrollable stress), the loss of dlPFC network connectivity, neuronal firing, and cognitive function (Fig. 4C).
D2 receptors have more limited expression in primate dlPFC and are focused in layer V neurons [120]. Alterations in D2 receptor stimulation, using compounds that interact with both D2 and D3 receptors, have little effect on Delay cell firing, but have large effects on the firing Response feedback neurons, cells that may respond to the corollary discharge that an eye movement as occurred [138]. Stimulation of D2/D3 receptors increases the amplitude [138] and speed [41] of the response, while blockade does the converse. As corollary discharge can be a neural signal of a self-generated movement or thought, distortions in D2R signaling may alter this operation and contribute to delusions and/or hallucinations, e.g., attributing actions to an outside agent. It should be noted that if the Delay cells and Response cells were added together, as is typical of most neurophysiological studies that average all neurons in a population response, we would observe that both D1R and D2R enhance working memory-related neuronal firing, albeit with the D1R enhancement limited to modest levels of D1R stimulation, while higher levels suppress firing.
Many of DA’s influences on dlPFC neuronal firing during spatial working memory can also be seen during other cognitive operations [131, 139], e.g., where high doses of D1R stimulation are suppressive (although “signals” can be maintained and accentuated if the dose is not too high), while D2R stimulation is often excitatory, especially in regard to motor responses [130]. For example, high levels of D1R stimulation render pyramidal cells more vulnerable to distraction [136]. It remains to be seen whether new, lower affinity D1R agonists will strengthen rather than weaken resistance to distraction, as suggested by experiments that deplete endogenous DA [67]. Recordings from dlPFC neurons in monkeys representing and evaluating numbers also show that DA can enhance rule encoding, where high levels of D1R stimulation suppress general neuronal firing but increase responses to the preferred rule, while D2R stimulation excited neuronal firing in general while suppressing responses to the nonpreferred rule [140]. D2R stimulation also enhanced persistent firing of numerical representations by dlPFC neurons [141]. It should be noted that the experiments on D2R mechanisms in primate dlPFC generally utilize the agonist, quinpirole, which stimulates D2, D3, and D4 receptors [142]. As D2R and D4R are concentrated on interneurons in dlPFC ([143], and see next paragraph), some of the excitatory effects of quinpirole in these studies may arise from D2R/D4R inhibition of interneurons, thus disinhibiting pyramidal cell firing [141].
The primate frontal eye fields (FEF) are situated immediately caudal to the dlPFC, and appear to be modulated by DA in a similar manner. These studies also found that D1R predominated on pyramidal cells compared to interneurons, while D2R predominated on interneurons [144]. D1R were particularly concentrated on pyramidal cells with long-range projections back to the visual extrastriate cortices, which coordinate eye movements with visual attention [144]. Blockade of DA D1R stimulation in the FEF enhanced FEF influence on V4 processing of visual stimuli, consistent with higher levels of D1R stimulation weakening neuronal function in both dlPFC and FEF [145]. Although these data suggest that DA D1R may have similar effects across cortical areas, it is likely that there are multiple differences as well, and thus extensive research will be needed to learn how DA influences distinct circuit operations across the primate cortex. Additional research is also needed to understand the contributions of D3, D4, and D5 receptors. It is noteworthy that the D4 receptor actually has higher affinity for NA than DA [146], a reminder that our terminology does not respect the overlap in catecholamine actions.
Noradrenaline neuromodulation in dlPFC
Although initial research on catecholamine actions in dlPFC focused on DA, it is now appreciated that NA has just as powerful effects on dlPFC physiology and function with a parallel, inverted U dose response (Fig. 5). Importantly, the separation of beneficial vs. detrimental actions at separate adrenergic receptor subtypes has allowed for rapid translation to human therapeutics. NA has differing affinities for its receptors, with highest affinity for α2A-AR, lower affinity for α1A-AR, and lowest affinity for β-AR [147]. Thus, the amount of NA release can determine which receptors are engaged, and whether dlPFC Delay cell firing is enhanced or weakened. As reviewed above, NA is released according to arousal state, with little or no LC firing during sleep, phasic firing to relevant stimuli during nonstress waking, and high levels of spontaneous firing during stress [36, 51, 52]. The data suggest that α2A-AR, but not α1-AR or β-AR, are predominately engaged during nonstressed waking when there are moderate levels of NA release [148], while α1A-AR become engaged with higher levels of NA release during stress exposure [149]. Although previous research has emphasized the presynaptic role of α2A-AR, it should be noted that the enhancing effects of NA in PFC occur at postsynaptic α2A-AR [107, 150, 151], which are localized on spines near glutamate synapses, positioned to strengthen connectivity [107]. As α2A-AR strengthen dlPFC network connectivity, and α1A-AR weaken connectivity and dlPFC firing, these changes in arousal state can cause large changes in cognitive state.
As illustrated in Fig. 5A, B, stimulation of α2A-AR under nonstress conditions strengthens dlPFC Delay cell firing via inhibition of cAMP-K+ channel signaling [107]. Conversely, blockade of α2A-AR markedly reduces Delay cell firing that can be reversed by blocking HCN channels [107, 152]. Similar effects have been seen on working memory performance with local or systemic administration of α2A-AR agonists or antagonists [148, 150, 153, 154]. The α2A-AR selective agonist, guanfacine, is able to improve working memory with the least sedative side effects [153, 155, 156], and also improves other PFC cognitive operations such as attention regulation [157], associative learning [158], delayed discounting [159], and reversal learning dependent on the ventral PFC [160]. Guanfacine is in widespread use for treating PFC disorders based on research in animals [161]. It is noteworthy that the enhancing effects of methylphenidate on working memory in monkeys involve increased α2A-AR, as well as D1R actions [9]. Thus, α2A-AR stimulation likely contributes to the increased stability of representations produced by methylphenidate in humans [135].
In contrast, stimulation of α1-AR reduces Delay cell firing and impairs working memory performance. As seen in Fig. 5C, iontophoresis of an α1-AR agonist into the dlPFC reduces Delay cell firing [162, 163], and local infusion of an α1-AR agonist into dlPFC impairs working memory performance [154]. Recent data suggest that lower levels of α1-AR stimulation may sometimes enhance dlPFC neuronal firing, and that this may be related to presynaptic α1-AR positioned to enhance release, and/or to modest increases in internal calcium release in spines [163]. However, higher levels of α1-AR stimulation produce profound decreases in dlPFC Delay cell firing (Fig. 5C), and this is mediated through calcium-PKC signaling [162]. Brief pulses of α1-AR stimulation may help to weaken existing network connections to promote network reconfiguration, e.g., in response to an unexpected event [164]. However, sustained high levels of α1-AR stimulation contribute to stress-induced PFC dysfunction [162]. These findings in animals help to explain why α1-AR antagonists such as prazosin are used to treat PTSD [165].
Less is known about how β-AR stimulation alters dlPFC Delay cell firing and dlPFC function. Infusion of a general β-AR antagonist into the monkey dlPFC had no effect on working memory under nonstress conditions, suggesting that these actions do not have a large influence during normal arousal conditions [148]. However, this could be an inaccurate conclusion if β1-AR and β2-AR have opposite effects on dlPFC function. Systemic administration of a β1-AR antagonist improves PFC working memory function in monkeys, suggesting that these receptors may have detrimental actions [166]. However, further research is needed to understand the role of β-AR in dlPFC.
Serotonin modulation of PFC
Little is known about serotonin’s cellular and molecular actions in primate dlPFC. Although researchers have found no effect of serotonin depletion in dlPFC on working memory performance [26, 27], more detailed dissection of serotonin receptor actions provides a different picture. Iontophoresis of a 5HT2A receptor agonist accentuated the spatial tuning of Delay cells by increasing firing for preferred target locations and/or reducing firing for nonpreferred locations, while an antagonist had converse actions [72]. These data emphasize that negative findings with depletion experiments do not mean that a modulator has no effect, but rather that there may be complex underlying receptor actions. Much greater research is needed in this area, especially given the lesion data indicating that serotonin is critical to ventral PFC function [27]. For example, how do different serotonin receptor subtypes alter the functioning of BA25, and are there specific receptors that can quiet BA25 activity and possibly have anti-depressant actions? This is an arena where marmoset research may be particularly helpful.
Overall summary of nonhuman primate dlPFC modulation
Overall, research on neuromodulators shows that they have a profound influence on dlPFC function in monkeys, with an inverted U dose response that is consistent with the changes in dlPFC function with alterations in arousal state (summarized in Fig. 6). A more nuanced understanding of the inverted U has also emerged, with the understanding that what an “optimal” neurochemical state depends on the cognitive operation being performed. For example, wider mental representations (e.g., strong α2A-AR, lower D1) may benefit operations where broader network connections are useful (e.g., creative integration), whereas more narrowed, precise representations (higher D1R) may benefit tasks requiring precision and focus (e.g., math problems). We have also learned that loss of regulation of these powerful signaling events, e.g., by genetic insults or with advancing age, contributes to dlPFC deficits in cognitive disorders (Fig. 3C), emphasizing their importance to human health.
Effects of monoaminergic and cholinergic manipulations of human PFC function
Research in humans necessarily involves global manipulations, such as systemic drug treatment, or studies of genotype, on cognitive functioning and brain activity patterns. Available pharmacological agents safe for human use often have nonselective actions, further complicating mechanistic interpretations. Nonetheless, the data indicate that neuromodulators have powerful effects on PFC cognitive functions in humans that can be consistent with that seen in monkeys. For example, muscarinic and Nic-α7R blockade impairs working memory in humans [167], as well as monkeys [93], emphasizing the importance of acetylcholine to dlPFC function. Similarly, methylphenidate, which increases both DA and NA availability in the PFC, enhances the stability of dlPFC representations, rendering them resistant to distractors [135], consistent with D1R and α2A-AR increasing dlPFC delay-related firing in monkeys described above. Indeed, the widespread use of the α2A-AR agonist, guanfacine, to treat PFC disorders such as ADHD, arose from this work in monkeys [161]. The following is a review of monoamine and cholinergic influences on human PFC cognitive functioning, where cognitive tasks can be more readily elaborated to assess a wider range of PFC functions.
Effects of DA on human PFC function
Experimental work with human participants has substantiated the necessity of DA for working memory and associated delay-period activity in the PFC [135, 168,169,170,171,172,173,174,175,176,177,178,179,180,181], as well as other cognitive functions such as future planning and cognitive flexibility [182,183,184,185].
A key challenge for human research on PFC DA function has been the limited availability of selective DA D1 receptor agents [186]. However, an important role of for D1R stimulation in human working memory is substantiated by PET imaging with D1R-selective ligands. For example, a simultaneous PET/fMRI study with the D1R-selective PET ligand [11C] NNC112, in 29 healthy volunteers by Roffman et al. [187] has revealed that cortical D1R density predicted working memory-related decoupling of frontoparietal and default networks, which respectively manage task-related and internal stimuli. Specifically, lower cortical D1R density was associated with greater decoupling. These findings concur with the hypothesis that cortical dopamine signaling controls network dynamics to redirect cognitive resources to working memory. Moreover, the findings echo neuromodulatory effects of D1R signaling at the level of cortical microcircuits. As reviewed above, in nonhuman primates, optimal levels of D1R receptor stimulation suppress task-irrelevant inputs onto dendritic spines through a variety of mechanisms, including excitation of fast-spiking interneurons and cAMP-mediated effects on HCN channels. An intriguing target for future research is the cellular basis of network (de)coupling by D1R stimulation during working memory.
These findings also substantiated earlier PET work with the same ligand showing that an increase in dlPFC D1R is associated with working memory impairment in schizophrenia (although not healthy volunteers), which was interpreted as a compensatory upregulation in response to chronic deficits in DA tone, but could also be a sign of excessive DA D1R signaling impairing working memory early in the disease process [188, 189]. Reduced D1R availability has also been linked to cognitive deficits with advancing age [190], although this may reflect the loss of spines in dlPFC with age [191], as this is the main site of D1R localization [121].
A role for D1R stimulation in working memory was also suggested by early work with the first generation of selective D1R agonists dihydrexidine (DAR-0100) or its active enantiomer (DAR-0100A) in patients with SCZ and SCZ spectrum disorders. This work showed that it increased perfusion of PFC and enhanced performance on classic working memory tasks, like the n-back [192]. Moreover, very recent work with novel selective partial, non-catechol D1 receptor agents that avoid the major side effects associated with prior D1R agonists (e.g., on blood pressure), like PF-0641256, has shown effects on cost-benefit decision making, reversal learning, and Pavlovian control of responding depending on baseline working memory capacity [186]. For example, higher doses of PF-06412562 improved reversal learning only in individuals with low baseline working memory capacity. This development opens avenues for further unraveling the precise computational and neural mechanisms of D1R effects on working memory in healthy volunteers. The evidence for inverted U-shaped effects also in humans further emphasize the importance of taking into account his individual variability when aiming to isolate these effects.
Nevertheless, it is fair to say that the majority of the evidence for DA’s role in modulating human working memory comes from studies investigating either nonselective catecholaminergic drugs, such as methylphenidate, or D2 family agents that alter D2, D3, and often D4 receptors. Thus, while nonhuman primate research has highlighted in particular a key role for the DA D1R in dlPFC mediation of working memory, human work reinforces observations from work with rodents that DA can also modulate PFC functions, including working memory, set-shifting and reversal learning, via modulating dopamine D2R [193]. One possibility is that pharmacological stimulation or blockade of D2R alters D1R stimulation indirectly, via action at presynaptic D2R, thus eliciting changes in autoregulation of synthesis and release leading to paradoxical effects on DA tone. However, the functional consequences of D1R and D2R stimulation, at least as measured in vitro in rat mPFC, are quite different [194]. According to dual-state theory of PFC DA [195], which is grounded in computational modeling of biophysical effects of DA in rodent medial PFC, the D1R mode, associated with intermediate levels of DA, is accompanied by distractor-resistant stabilization of working memory representations, while the D2R mode, associated with either very low or very high DA levels, is accompanied by flexible updating of working memory representations. This hypothesis is supported by pharmacological neuroimaging work with human volunteers, showing that D2R stimulation, with the D2R agonists bromocriptine or cabergoline, indeed reduces the distractor-resistance of working memory representations by disrupting delay activity in dlPFC, selectively after distraction (Fig. 7) [168]. This impairment may reflect either stimulation of postsynaptic D2 receptors, shifting the PFC into a flexible D2R mode, and/or, as described above, reduced DA release and D1R stimulation in dlPFC due to bromocriptine actions at autoreceptors.
As is the case for D1R effects in nonhuman primates, so do effects of DA D2R drugs in humans also depend on the baseline state of the agent [196,197,198,199,200]. For example, the effects of bromocriptine and similar agonists on PFC function interact with the subject’s baseline working memory ability with the drug improving cognition in subjects with lower baseline working memory abilities in the “un-drugged” state, while worsening cognition in those with higher baseline working memory capacity [169, 171, 173, 180, 201]. Such span-dependent drug effects are seen not just for classic dlPFC tasks, such as those measuring set-shifting, working memory updating [202, 203], and working memory retrieval, but also for tasks measuring functions more commonly associated with (ventral) striatal regions, such as reinforcement learning [184, 203] and Pavlovian biasing of instrumental responding [204]. Thus, effects of DArgic drugs can, at least partly, be predicted from the initial state of the individual, an observation with clear clinical implications for individual tailoring of DArgic drug therapy.
What is the mechanistic basis of this interaction between baseline state and DA drug effects on working memory capacity in humans? It likely relates to the endogenous levels of DA neurotransmission, e.g., where those with inadequate endogenous DA levels and weaker cognition are helped by drug, and those with optimal endogenous DA and cognition are impaired by drug treatment. This follows from findings showing that effects of DArgic drugs, like the D2 receptor agonist bromocriptine, the D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride, the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) inhibitor tolcapone, and the catecholamine reuptake blocker methylphenidate, can all be predicted from baseline levels of DA, as indexed with [18F]FDOPA or [18F]FMT PET [200, 205, 206], or by genetic differences between individuals, e.g., in the Val158Met polymorphism in the COMT gene or the Taq1A polymorphism of the DRD2 gene [134, 207,208,209,210].
Where DA-induced improvements of spatial tuning during working memory are accompanied by suppressive effects on dlPFC activity in nonhuman primates [128], so is working memory improvement after DA-enhancing drug administration in human functional imaging studies accompanied by reductions in dlPFC activity [133, 207, 211, 212]. Moreover, where too much D1 receptor stimulation in nonhuman primates, e.g., due to stress, can elicit a quelling of PFC activity and a blocking of all new input to the PFC, thus leading to perseverative responding [123, 125, 127], so can (nonspecific) dopaminergic drug administration in human subjects with already high levels of dlPFC DA also promote perseverative responding and maladaptive, inefficient increases in BOLD signaling [207]. Findings from two small sample DA PET studies demonstrating a positive link between working memory capacity and baseline DA synthesis capacity, as measured with [18F]FMT PET imaging [213, 214] further substantiate the hypothesis that the interaction between DA drug effects on dlPFC function and working memory span reflects an inverted U-shaped relationship with individual differences in baseline levels of striatal DA synthesis capacity, although studies with larger sample sizes are required to substantiate the evidence for such a link.
The dual-state theory of prefrontal cortex by Durstewitz and Seamans (2008) might also relevant here when accounting for the interaction between baseline dopamine levels and DA drug effects, given that D1 and D2 receptors have been suggested to have differential (respectively less and more) affinity [215] with opposite functional effects [194, 195]. A more recent biophysically realistic model that takes into account a D1R gradient across the rostrocaudal hierarchy in PFC (with more D1R in more rostral PFC) captures the inverted U-shaped dependence of working memory on DA as well as the filtering of irrelevant stimuli by enhancing inhibition of pyramidal cell dendrites [216]. This filtering may also occur through D1R-cAMP-K+ channel gating on spines (see above), as pyramidal cells and spines are the predominant location for DA synapses and D1R in human as well as monkey PFC [121, 217]. The rostral-caudal gradient is particularly intriguing given that more rostral PFC areas have been implicated in maintaining more abstract representations [218, 219]. Thus, rostral D1R stimulation might be in a perfect position to strengthen the most abstract goals, while also allowing flexible updating at a lower level of the computational hierarchy.
A final key point is that, through top-down control, the PFC can shape the responses of DA neurons in the midbrain and DA release [220,221,222]. Just as rodent medial and orbitofrontal cortex shape signals of midbrain DA neurons [223, 224], so are BOLD responses in human striatum shaped by an internal model of the task environment in working memory, putatively computed at the level of cortex [225,226,227]. In humans, there is also direct evidence for a role of the PFC in controlling DA release. This evidence comes from work combining transcranial magnetic stimulation with [11C]raclopride PET, even showing topographical specificity, with DA release in specific subregions of the striatum being controlled by regions in PFC that are strongly connected with those striatal regions in a topographically selective manner [228,229,230,231].
Effects of noradrenaline on human PFC function
The LC NA system has been proposed, like cortical DA, to contribute to working memory with moderate (phasic firing) levels of NA strengthening representations held in memory stores and high (tonic firing) levels of NA weakening them [147, 232]. This is likely due to differing degrees of NA release engaging with different subtypes of adrenergic receptors that have opposing effects on dlPFC connectivity and neuronal firing. As summarized in Fig. 5, iontophoretic recordings from monkey dlPFC indicate that lower level, phasic NA release engages high affinity α2A-AR to strengthen dlPFC recurrent excitatory connections underlying mental representations [107], while higher tonic levels of NA release would engage lower affinity α1-AR and reduce weaken connectivity and representations [163].
In concert with this proposal are data from studies with humans showing that the α2A-AR agonist, guanfacine, can improve a variety of PFC cognitive operations in both healthy human subjects and patients with PFC deficits (reviewed in [161]), while the α1-AR antagonist, prazosin, is used to treat PTSD (reviewed in [165]). Guanfacine is able to restore PFC top-down control over attention, action, and emotion [233, 234], and recent work finds that guanfacine can facilitate emergence from delirium [235], consistent with α2A-AR actions increasing PFC network connectivity [107]. Guanfacine treatment improves the ability to resist distraction in monkeys [157, 236] and humans [237], similar to methylphenidate [135], raising the possibility that the enhancing effects of methylphenidate involve increased endogenous α2A-AR stimulation, as seen in monkeys [9]. The NA reuptake blocker, atomoxetine, has also been shown to increase the functional connectivity of the dlPFC in relationship to working memory capacity [238], although this compound increases DA as well as NA levels in the PFC [239]. Recordings from monkeys show that atomoxetine has an inverted U dose response on delay cell firing, with low doses enhancing firing via enhanced α2A-AR stimulation, but slightly higher levels reducing firing via increased α1-AR stimulation [9]. These mixed effects may help to explain some of the complexities in the literature using this compound.
Moreover, these data have relevance for the adaptive gain theory, according to which different modes of NA transmission regulate the tradeoff between exploitation and exploration [240]. In this model, a phasic mode promotes exploitative behavior and focused attention by facilitating processing of task-relevant information, where top-down control may come from dlPFC inputs [30]. In contrast, increasing the tonic mode promotes behavioral disengagement and divided attention, thus allowing potentially new and more rewarding behaviors to be explored. In the model, the transition from the phasic to the tonic NA mode is controlled by specific regions in the PFC, e.g., the orbitofrontal cortex and/or the ACC, which in turn control the firing of NA neurons in the brainstem in a top-down manner.
Arnsten et al. showed that the spines of layer III dlPFC pyramidal cell networks express the molecular machinery to allow rapid changes in network connection strength (termed Dynamic Network Connectivity) by which neuromodulators such as NA can flexibly alter network configurations, e.g., based on environmental demands [100, 101, 241]. Although this work was based on physiological recordings in monkeys, recent fMRI studies in humans also show that enhanced NA signaling with atomoxetine can alter cortical network organization during the resting state [242], or during working memory [238]. This work has demonstrated that atomoxetine reorganizes the functional connectome as a function of cognitive demands, altering the balance between network-level segregation and integration [242]. Atomoxetine potentiated network segregation during rest, but, potentiated network integration during a classic n-back working memory task. Such findings have general relevance, e.g., in the context of acute stress, where stress-induced increases in NA activity have been argued to prompt large-scale neural network reconfiguration [243, 244] (and see below), consistent with the dynamic changes in synaptic strength based on α2A-AR vs. α1-AR predominant actions.
These findings are also reminiscent of earlier theoretical and modeling work, suggesting that NA acts as a “neural interrupt signal” that alerts the learner to an unpredicted change in the learning environment [245] and performs a “network reset” [164]. Noradrenergic “neural interruption” also surfaces as improved response inhibition in stop-signal tasks [246], which is accompanied by increases in connectivity of the inferior frontal cortex, reflecting in part greater sensitivity to afferent inputs [247,248,249,250]. The enhanced connectivity within the inferior PFC following atomoxetine may arise from increased NA stimulation of α2A-AR strengthening synaptic efficacy (see above), as atomoxetine enhances dlPFC neuronal firing via α2A-AR actions [9]. Consistent with this hypothesis, the α2A-AR agonist, guanfacine, improves stop-signal performance in monkeys [251], and in abstinent cocaine addicts [252], and improves impulse control in monkeys [159] and patients with ADHD [253].
The notion that increased (tonic firing) cortical NA is particularly important for explorative modes of behavior concurs with recent neural models [254] suggesting that noradrenergic increases in cognitive flexibility can be accounted for by changes in the uncertainty of an internal model of the environment [240, 245]. This account is grounded in the observation that environments characterized by high volatility benefit from higher levels of perceived uncertainty (and cognitive flexibility) than do stable environments. In line with this hypothesis, the β-AR-blocker, propranolol, used to reduce anxiety and blood pressure, affects how people responded to stable situations and to changeable ones [255]. Specifically, propranolol made participants more likely to rely on their expectations, based on prior experience with two co-occurring sensations, an effect that might contribute to its anxiolytic potential. However, it also led to slower learning to adjust these expectations when contingencies were uncertain and volatile/changeable. Thus, high levels of NA, e.g., acting through β1-AR and α1-AR, may weaken established patterns of network connectivity [101], promoting new conformations needed for the updating of our beliefs based on environmental uncertainty, allowing us to be more flexible in our constantly changing world.
In summary, both DA and NA play key roles in working memory, top-down control, and cognitive flexibility. The DA and NA inverted U dose-response curves seen in animals (Figs 4–6) also apply in humans [132], with moderate levels enhancing PFC functions, while high levels of catecholamine release, e.g., during stress, impairing dlPFC functions [256]. The separation of beneficial vs. detrimental NA actions by receptor subtype (α2A-AR vs. α1-AR respectively) has speeded the successful translation of therapeutics from animals to humans, while the D1R inverted U dose-response at the same receptors will remain a challenge. While it is clear that there is considerable specificity when it comes to these different neuromodulators, the precise nature of this specificity remains to be elucidated in humans. The use of more selective receptor agents, like the novel non-catechol D1 receptor selective and D2 receptor selective agents [206], ideally in pretreatment designs [184], and direct comparison of noradrenergic vs. DAergic agents (such as sulpiride vs. propranolol [257, 258]), should help to disentangle their likely complementary roles in future studies. There are certainly hints in the literature, with, e.g., NA subserving value-free, random forms of exploration [259] vs. DA subserving more informed, directed forms of exploration [260]. However, it will be important to remember that DA and NA have differing effects through different receptors, and that many compounds available for human use are nonselective. In particular, there is a great need for selective, low affinity D1R agonists, and selective D1R antagonists, that are safe to use in humans, to truly understand DA actions on human cognitive circuits, particularly as this receptor predominates in PFC circuits.
Effects of acetylcholine on human PFC function
DA and NA are not the only neuromodulators that modulate PFC function, such as attention shifting, behavioral flexibility and working memory. ACh is also released in the PFC upon the presentation of a salient target, and pharmacological manipulations of ACh in humans have also revealed inverted U-shaped relationship with task performance [261] (see above). Cholinergic receptor stimulation in PFC is thought to elicit an attentional shift akin to Posner’s attentional orienting response, in order to align attention with a source of sensory input. Much of the recent cognitive work on ACh in humans has focused on perceptual representations in visual cortex. For example, cholinergic enhancement with the cholinesterase inhibitor donepezil decreases the spread of early visual cortical excitatory fMRI responses to a visual stimulus [262] consistent with cholinergic receptor enrichment on interneurons in V1 [263], and improves detection of a target flanked by distractors, consistent with sharpened visuospatial perceptual representations [264], and in line with cholinergic agents reducing responses to distractors in monkey dlPFC neurons [265]. Similar effects were not seen after administration of bromocriptine (a DA D2/D1 receptor agonist) or guanfacine (a noradrenergic α2A-AR agonist) [264], suggesting a specific role of acetylcholine in sharpening perceptual representations.
In addition, cholinergic modulation also influences human working memory. For example, blockade of muscarinic and nicotinic receptors impairs working memory performance [167], while a nicotinic agonist can improve working memory in both non-smokers and abstinent smokers [266, 267]. Nonspecific enhancement of cholinergic signaling can also enhance the processing of the posterior cortices, e.g., sensory input from the thalamus to the visual cortices, but high doses can weaken top-down internal models held online by the PFC [261, 268]. In contrast to NA manipulations, cholinergic manipulations generally leave extra-dimensional set-shifting unaffected. Conversely, cholinergic changes in rodents are associated with attentional shifts in Posner-like attention orienting paradigms where subjects are aware of cue invalidity [269]. These involve parietal attentional mechanisms [270] that are impaired by cholinergic lesions in monkeys [271]. Moreover late (but not early) reversal learning is sensitive to manipulation of ACh, but not NA [66, 246]. Thus, the signals that trigger this NA- and Ach-mediated flexibility might differ. As suggested by Yu and Dayan (2005): NA might be involved predominantly when changes in the environment are “unexpected,” whereas ACh might instead signal “expected” uncertainty, which arises from known unreliability of predictive relationships within a familiar environment [272].
Effects of serotonin on human PFC function
Functions associated with the dlPFC, such as spatial working memory and attentional set-shifting, have been the focus of only few human studies on serotonin. Early work revealed that the serotonin releaser fenfluramine impaired spatial delayed response task performance in contrast to the DA agonist bromocriptine, which facilitated spatial memory [177]. Decrements in working memory performance have been observed after increases in the serotonin precursor tryptophan [273] and theoretical work has highlighted similar nonlinear complexities as for the DA system [274]. As is the case for nonhuman primate studies, human studies have revealed that functions associated with the medial and orbital frontal cortex, such as probabilistic reversal learning tasks, are particularly sensitive to serotonergic manipulation or stratification [246, 275, 276]. Conversely, acute tryptophan depletion does not affect attentional set-shifting [277, 278]. In line with these observations, the majority of studies on serotonin’s role in human cognition and behavior have focused on affective (hardwired aversive) biases of learning and decision making, often as a function of delay, risk or effort costs [279,280,281,282,283,284]. Serotoninergic changes in aversive biases can surface as changes in inhibitory tendencies [285,286,287]. For example, under basal conditions, humans respond more slowly to an aversive stimulus than a rewarding stimulus, but following tryptophan depletion, they respond equally quickly to both [285]. However, unlike DA or NA, manipulating global serotonin levels does not affect performance on tasks of inhibition that have no clear affective component, such as the stop-signal reaction time task [246, 288,289,290,291], and the go/nogo task [292, 293] (but see [294]). These findings are consistent with what is seen in monkeys, where serotonin depletion alters the functioning of orbital PFC, but not dlPFC (see above). These associations reflect serotonin’s implications in disorders of impulsivity and depression, but the jury is still out regarding the direction of serotonin’s effects, or the selectivity to the aversive domain [295]. It is likely that the effects vary between subcortical and cortical target systems [296], e.g., as a function of environmental threat levels [297] and/or the degree to which stressors are controllable [298]. It is also likely that manipulations that influence selective serotonergic receptors may have a very different, and larger effect than global changes in serotonin availability, but these compounds are rarely available for human use. How these compounds alter medial PFC and cingulate functioning may have particular relevance to the etiology and treatment of mood disorders, and will be especially important for future research.
Summary of human data
This review of the effects of manipulating the monoamine and cholinergic systems on human cognition reveals a number of general principles of chemical neuromodulation that have also been noted in computational theories of neuromodulation [299, 300]. First, manipulation of these large ascending neuromodulators has radically different functional consequences depending on where they act in the brain. Second, all these systems exhibit nonlinear Inverted U shape functions and baseline dependency: drugs that increase receptor stimulation having positive effects in systems with low baseline levels of activity, but negative effects in systems with high baseline levels of activity. While such nonlinearity is a feature of any system that is geared toward self-regulation or homeostasis (including body temperature and home central heating), it is particularly apt for the neuromodulation of the PFC, where inverted U dose-response curves are often remarkably narrow, creating a challenge for pharmacological treatments. The implication of this observation is that isolation of drug effects on human cognition requires that the baseline state of the system is taken into account. Finally, release of and responsiveness to these neuromodulators are all regulated not only by the activity of the neurons in the brainstem, but also locally in their target brain regions. This allows the impact of these neuromodulatory systems to be controlled in a top-down manner by advanced cortical computations of current beliefs about the environment. Future studies might address the obvious next question whether our remarkable capacity to adapt flexibly to our constantly changing environment stems in part from this ability to prioritize distinct neurochemical projection systems in a manner that is tailored to the current perceived environment.
Future directions and clinical implications
Research in both human and nonhuman primates finds that the arousal neuromodulators have powerful and complex effects on PFC circuits, often with different effects based on amount of release, and with different cognitive operations having distinct neurochemical needs. Most modulators have a narrow inverted U dose-response influence on PFC functions, which coordinates arousal and cognitive states. However, these actions are often the cause of cognitive deficits, especially as stress potentiates mental disorders, and have complicated the translation of potential therapeutics from basic to clinical research. Additional pharmacological tools (receptor selective, low affinity matching endogenous transmitter actions) are needed to better understand receptor actions, and to allow broader inverted U dose-response curves, which may facilitate therapeutic strategies. There are many outstanding questions in this field, especially as research to date in nonhuman primates as only been performed in a few labs, and focused on just a few PFC subregions. Important, outstanding questions include:
-
What are the roles of neuromodulators in dlPFC layers beyond layer III and on GABA interneurons?
-
How do neuromodulators impact the functioning of PFC areas beyond the dlPFC? In particular, what are the roles of various 5HT receptors on neuronal firing in BA25 and on stress-related computations?
-
How do the molecular cellular signaling mechanisms and the computational features of DA’s effects on working memory and the cortical hierarchy differ from those of NA and how are the various systems controlled/prioritized?
-
How do the various neuromodulators implicated in working memory and goal-directed behavior interact synergistically and/or competitively?
-
Can we expand our use of genetic tools in the primate to produce circuit and cell-specific manipulations, similar to the powerful studies now being done in rodent? These new technologies also allow visualization of neuromodulator release. Can we create transgenic monkeys to have a better understanding of how mutations associated with cognitive disorders alter the circuits that mediate higher cognition?
-
Can we develop new methods to target drugs to specific brain regions in humans? Can we find unique signatures of PFC subcircuits that might allow nanoparticle targeting of drug deliver?
-
Can we begin to understand the neuromodulation of PFC metacognitive operations such as insight, which are impaired by uncontrollable stress [301], often at great cost to society, e.g., as is evident in the COVID pandemic. Might these capabilities, generated by the frontal pole, be modulated in a similar of differing fashion to the dlPFC?
Given the power of neuromodulatory influences on the primate PFC, understanding these mechanisms will be essential to revealing the etiology of many cognitive disorders, and hopes for superior therapeutics.
References
Berger B, Gaspar P, Verney C. Dopaminergic innervation of the cerebral cortex: unexpected differences between rodents and primates. Trends Neurosci. 1991;14:21–27.
Coppola JJ, Disney AA. Is there a canonical cortical circuit for the cholinergic system? Anatomical differences across common model systems. Front Neural Circuits. 2018;12:8.
Wise SP. Forward frontal fields: phylogeny and fundamental function. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31:599–608.
Wallis CU, Cardinal RN, Alexander L, Roberts AC, Clarke HF. Opposing roles of primate areas 25 and 32 and their putative rodent homologs in the regulation of negative emotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2017;114:E4075–84.
Selemon LD, Rajkowska G, Goldman-Rakic PS. Abnormally high neuronal density in the schizophrenic cortex: a morphometric analysis of prefrontal area 9 and occipital area 17. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52:805–18.
Glantz LA, Lewis DA. Decreased dendritic spine density on prefrontal cortical pyramidal neurons in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:65–73.
Hof PR, Morrison JH. Neocortical neuronal subpopulations labeled by a monoclonal antibody to calbindin exhibit differential vulnerability in Alzheimer’s disease. Exp Neurol. 1991;111:293–301.
Berridge CW, Devilbiss DM, Andrzejewski ME, Arnsten AFT, Kelley AE, Schmeichel B, et al. Methylphenidate preferentially increases catecholamine neurotransmission within the prefrontal cortex at low doses that enhance cognitive function. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:1111–20.
Gamo NJ, Wang M, Arnsten AFT. Methylphenidate and atomoxetine improve prefrontal cortical function via noradrenergic alpha-2 and dopaminergic D1 receptor stimulation. J Am Acad Child Adol Psychiatry. 2010;49:1011–23.
Schaeffer DJ, Gilbert KM, Gati JS, Menon RS, Everling S. Intrinsic functional boundaries of lateral frontal cortex in the common marmoset monkey. J Neurosci. 2019;39:1020–9.
Preuss T. Critique of pure marmoset. Brain Behav Evol. 2019;93:1–6.
Azuar C, Reyes P, Slachevsky A, Volle E, Kinkingnehun S, Kouneiher F, et al. Testing the model of caudo-rostral organization of cognitive control in the human with frontal lesions. Neuroimage. 2014;84:1053–60.
Fleming SM, Huijgen J, Dolan RJ. Prefrontal contributions to metacognition in perceptual decision making. J Neurosci. 2012;32:6117–25.
Tsujimoto S, Genovesio A, Wise SP. Frontal pole cortex: encoding ends at the end of the endbrain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:169–76.
Goldman-Rakic PS. Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1988;11:137–56.
Ongür D, Price JL. The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:206–19.
Suzuki M, Gottlieb J. Distinct neural mechanisms of distractor suppression in the frontal and parietal lobe. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:98–104.
Buschman TJ, Miller EK. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science. 2007;315:1860–2.
Schall JD, Boucher L. Executive control of gaze by the frontal lobes. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7:396–412.
Small DM, Zatorre RJ, Dagher A, Evans AC, Jones-Gotman M. Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversion. Brain. 2001;124:1720–33.
Rudebeck PH, Saunders RC, Lundgren DA, Murray EA. Specialized representations of value in the orbital and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: desirability versus availability of outcomes. Neuron. 2017;95:1208–20.
Barbas H, Pandya DN. Architecture and intrinsic connections of the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1989;286:353–75.
Joyce MKP, Barbas H. Cortical connections position primate area 25 as a keystone for interoception, emotion, and memory. J Neurosci. 2018;38:1677–98.
Barbas H, Joyce MK, Garcia-Cabezas MA, John Y. Serial prefrontal pathways are positioned to balance cognition and emotion in primates. J Neurosci. 2020;40:8306–28.
Chafee MV, Goldman-Rakic PS. Inactivation of parietal and prefrontal cortex reveals interdependence of neural activity during memory-guided saccades. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:1550–66.
Brozoski T, Brown RM, Rosvold HE, Goldman PS. Cognitive deficit caused by regional depletion of dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey. Science. 1979;205:929–31.
Clarke HF, Walker SC, Crofts HS, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Prefrontal serotonin depletion affects reversal learning but not attentional set shifting. J Neurosci. 2005;25:532–8.
Walker SC, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Differential contributions of dopamine and serotonin to orbitofrontal cortex function in the marmoset. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:889–98.
Roberts AC. The importance of serotonin for orbitofrontal function. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:1185–91.
Arnsten AFT, Goldman-Rakic PS. Selective prefrontal cortical projections to the region of the locus coeruleus and raphe nuclei in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res. 1984;306:9–18.
Williams SM, Goldman-Rakic PS. Widespread origin of the primate mesofrontal dopamine system. Cereb Cortex. 1998;8:321–45.
Unguez GA, Schneider JS. Dopaminergic dorsal raphe neurons in cats and monkeys are sensitive to the toxic effects of MPTP. Neurosci Lett. 1988;94:218–23.
Wilson MA, Molliver ME. The organization of serotonergic projections to cerebral cortex in primates: retrograde transport studies. Neuroscience. 1991;44:555–70.
Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ, Levey AI, Wainer BH. Cholinergic innervation of cortex by the basal forebrain: cytochemistry and cortical connections of the septal area, diagonal band nuclei, nucleus basalis (substantia innominata), and hypothalamus in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1983;214:170–97.
Chandler DJ, Gao WJ, Waterhouse BD. Heterogeneous organization of the locus coeruleus projections to prefrontal and motor cortices. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:6816–21.
Saper CB, Fuller PM. Wake-sleep circuitry: an overview. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2017;44:186–92.
Lewis DA, Cambell MJ, Foote SL, Goldstein M, Morrison JH. The distribution of tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive fibers in primate neocortex is widespread but regionally specific. J Neurosci. 1987;282:317–30.
Berger B, Trottier S, Verney C, Gasper P, Alvarez C. Regional and laminar distribution of the dopamine and serotonin innervation in the macaque cerebral cortex: a radioautographic study. J Comp Neurol. 1988;273:99–119.
Lewis DA, Morrison JH. Noradrenergic innervation of monkey prefrontal cortex: a dopamine-beta-hydroxylase immunohistochemical study. J Comp Neurol. 1989;282:317–30.
Williams SM, Goldman-Rakic PS. Characterization of the dopaminergic innervation of the primate frontal cortex using a dopamine-specific antibody. Cereb Cortex. 1993;3:199–222.
Arnsten AFT, Wang M, Paspalas CD. Dopamine’s actions in primate prefrontal cortex: challenges for treating cognitive disorders. Pharmacol Rev. 2015;67:681–96.
Chiba T, Kayahara K, Nakano K. Efferent projections of infralimbic and prelimbic areas of the medial prefrontal cortex in the Japanese monkey, Macaca fuscata. Brain Res. 2001;888:83–101.
Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ. Neural inputs into the nucleus basalis of the substantia innominata (Ch4) in the rhesus monkey. Brain. 1984;107:253–74.
Frankle WG, Laruelle M, Haber SN. Prefrontal cortical projections to the midbrain in primates: evidence for a sparse connection. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:1627–36.
Price JL, Amaral DG. An autoradiographic study of the projections of the central nucleus of the monkey amygdala. J Neurosci. 1981;1:1242–59.
Van Bockstaele EJ, Colago EE, Valentino RJ. Amygdaloid corticotropin-releasing factor targets locus coeruleus dendrites: substrate for the co-ordination of emotional and cognitive limbs of the stress response. J Neuroendocrinol. 1998;10:743–57.
Foote SL, Aston-Jones G, Bloom FE. Impulse activity of locus coeruleus neurons in awake rats and monkeys is a function of sensory stimulation and arousal. PNAS. 1980;77:3033–7.
Grant SJ, Redmond DEJ. Neuronal activity of the locus ceruleus in awake Macaca arctoides. Exp Neurol. 1984;84:701–8.
Richardson RT, DeLong MR. Nucleus basalis of Meynert neuronal activity during a delayed response task in monkey. Brain Res. 1986;399:364–8.
Aston-Jones G, Rajkowski J, Cohen J. Role of locus coeruleus in attention and behavioral flexibility. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:1309–20.
Rajkowski J, Kubiak P, Inanova S, Aston-Jones G. State-related activity, reactivity of locus coeruleus neurons in behaving monkeys. Adv Pharm. 1998;42:740–44.
Rajkowski J, Majczynski H, Clayton E, Aston-Jones G. Activation of monkey locus coeruleus neurons varies with difficulty and performance in a target detection task. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92:361–71.
Bouret S, Richmond BJ. Sensitivity of locus ceruleus neurons to reward value for goal-directed actions. J Neurosci. 2015;35:4005–14.
Zhang K, Chen CD, Monosov IE. Novelty, salience, and surprise timing are signaled by neurons in the basal forebrain. Curr Biol. 2019;29:134–42.
Sara SJ, Bouret S. Orienting and reorienting: the locus coeruleus mediates cognition through arousal. Neuron. 2012;76:130–41.
Varazzani C, San-Galli A, Gilardeau S, Bouret S. Noradrenaline and dopamine neurons in the reward/effort trade-off: a direct electrophysiological comparison in behaving monkeys. J Neurosci. 2015;35:7866–77.
Hayashi K, Nakao K, Nakamura K. Appetitive and aversive information coding in the primate dorsal raphé nucleus. J Neurosci. 2015;35:6195–208.
Peroutka SJ, Lebovitz RM, Snyder SH. Two distinct central serotonin receptors with different physiological functions. Science. 1981;212:827–9.
Schultz W, Apicella P, Ljungberg T. Responses of monkey dopamine neurons to reward and conditioned stimuli during successive steps of learning a delayed response task. J Neurosci. 1993;13:900–13.
Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O. Two types of dopamine neuron distinctly convey positive and negative motivational signals. Nature. 2009;459:837–41.
Bromberg-Martin ES, Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O. Dopamine in motivational control: rewarding, aversive, and alerting. Neuron. 2010;68:815–34.
Kodama T, Hikosaka K, Honda Y, Kojima T, Watanabe M. Higher dopamine release induced by less rather than more preferred reward during a working memory task in the primate prefrontal cortex. Behav Brain Res. 2014;266:104–7.
Lammel S, Ion DI, Roeper J, Malenka RC. Projection-specific modulation of dopamine neuron synapses by aversive and rewarding stimuli. Neuron. 2011;70:855–62.
Wittmann MK, Fouragnan E, Folloni D, Klein-Flügge MC, Chau BKH, Khamassi M, et al. Global reward state affects learning and activity in raphe nucleus and anterior insula in monkeys. Nat Commun. 2020;11:3771.
Matias S, Lottem E, Dugué GP, Mainen ZF. Activity patterns of serotonin neurons underlying cognitive flexibility. Elife. 2017;6:e20552.
Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Differential regulation of fronto-executive function by the monoamines and acetylcholine. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:i151–60. Suppl 1
Crofts HS, Dalley JW, Collins P, van Denderen JCM, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW, et al. Differential effects of 6-OHDA lesions of the prefrontal cortex and caudate nucleus on the ability to acquire an attentional set. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:1015–26.
Croxson PL, Kyriazis DA, Baxter MG. Cholinergic modulation of a specific memory function of prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:1510–2.
Clarke HF, Walker SC, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion is behaviorally and neurochemically specific. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:18–27.
Ridley RM, Baker HF, Drewett B, Johnson JA. Effects of ibotinic acid lesions of the basal forebrain on serial reversal learning in marmosets. Psychopharmacol. 1985;86:438–43.
Roberts AC, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ, Muir JL. A specific form of cognitive rigidity following excitotoxic lesions of the basal forebrain in marmosets. Neuroscience. 1992;47:251–64.
Williams GV, Rao SG, Goldman-Rakic PS. The physiological role of 5-HT2A receptors in working memory. J Neurosci. 2002;22:2843–54.
Funahashi S, Bruce CJ, Goldman-Rakic PS. Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey’s dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1989;61:331–49.
Funahashi S, Chafee MV, Goldman-Rakic PS. Prefrontal neuronal activity in rhesus monkeys performing a delayed anti-saccade task. Nature. 1993;365:753–56.
Panichello MF, Buschman TJ. Shared mechanisms underlie the control of working memory and attention. Nature. 2021;592:601–5.
Fuster JM. Unit activity in prefrontal cortex during delayed response performance: neuronal correlates of transient memory. J Neurophysiol. 1973;36:61–78.
Fuster JM, Alexander GE. Neuron activity related to short-term memory. Science. 1971;173:652–4.
Goldman-Rakic PS. Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron. 1995;14:477–85.
González-Burgos G, Barrionuevo G, Lewis DA. Horizontal synaptic connections in monkey prefrontal cortex: an in vitro electrophysiological study. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:82–92.
González-Burgos G, Krimer LS, Povysheva NV, Barrionuevo G, Lewis DA. Functional properties of fast spiking interneurons and their synaptic connections with pyramidal cells in primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2005;93:942–53.
Torres-Gomez S, Blonde J, Mendoza-Halliday D, Kuebler E, Everest M, Wang X-J, et al. Changes in the proportion of inhibitory interneuron types from sensory to executive areas of the primate neocortex. Implications for the origins of working memory representations. Cereb Cortex. 2020;30:4544–62.
Bastos AM, Loonis R, Kornblith S, Lundqvist M, Miller EK. Laminar recordings in frontal cortex suggest distinct layers for maintenance and control of working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2018;115:1117–22.
Constantinidis C, Funahashi S, Lee D, Murray JD, Qi XL, Wang M, et al. Persistent spiking activity underlies working memory. J Neurosci. 2018;38:7020–8.
Yang ST, Wang M, Paspalas CP, Crimins JL, Altman MT, Mazer JA, et al. Core differences in synaptic signaling between primary visual and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28:1458–71.
Self MW, Kooijmans RN, Supèr H, Lamme VA, Roelfsema PR. Different glutamate receptors convey feedforward and recurrent processing in macaque V1. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:11031–6.
Nicoll RA. Expression mechanisms underlying long-term potentiation: a postsynaptic view. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003;358:721–6.
Gilman JP, Medalla M, Luebke JI. Area-specific features of pyramidal neurons-a comparative study in mouse and rhesus monkey. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27:2078–94.
Petralia RS, Wang YX, Hua F, Yi Z, Zhou A, Ge L, et al. Organization of NMDA receptors at extrasynaptic locations. Neuroscience. 2010;167:68–87.
Lin R, Duan Z, Sun H, Fung ML, Chen H, Wang J, et al. Kinesin-1 regulates extrasynaptic targeting of NMDARs and neuronal vulnerability toward excitotoxicity. iScience. 2019;13:82–97.
Wang M, Yang Y, Wang CJ, Gamo NJ, Jin LE, Mazer JA, et al. NMDA receptors subserve working memory persistent neuronal firing In dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2013;77:736–49.
Wang XJ. Synaptic basis of cortical persistent activity: the importance of NMDA receptors to working memory. J Neurosci. 1999;19:9587–603.
Yang Y, Paspalas CD, Jin LE, Picciotto MR, Arnsten AFT, Wang M. Nicotinic α7 receptors enhance NMDA cognitive circuits in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:12078–83.
Galvin VC, Yang S-T, Paspalas CD, Yang Y, Jin LE, Datta D, et al. Muscarinic M1 receptors modulate working memory performance and activity via KCNQ potassium channels in primate prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2020;106:649–61.
Major AJ, Vijayraghavan S, Everling S. Muscarinic attenuation of mnemonic rule representation in macaque dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during a pro- and anti-saccade task. J Neurosci. 2015;35:16064–76.
Major AJ, Vijayraghavan S, Everling S. Cholinergic overstimulation attenuates rule selectivity in macaque prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2018;38:1137–50.
Vijayraghavan S, Major AJ, Everling S. Muscarinic M1 receptor overstimulation disrupts working memory activity for rules in primate prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2018;98:1256–68.
Gao C, Wolf ME. Dopamine receptors regulate NMDA receptor surface expression in prefrontal cortex neurons. J Neurochem. 2008;106:2489–501.
Li YC, Liu G, Hu JL, Gao WJ, Huang YQ. Dopamine D(1) receptor-mediated enhancement of NMDA receptor trafficking requires rapid PKC-dependent synaptic insertion in the prefrontal neurons. J Neurochem. 2010;114:62–73.
Skeberdis VA, Chevaleyre V, Lau CG, Goldberg JH, Pettit DL, Suadicani SO, et al. Protein kinase A regulates calcium permeability of NMDA receptors. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:501–10.
Arnsten AFT, Datta D, Wang M. The genie in the bottle—magnified calcium signaling in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Mol Psychiatry. 2020. (online ahead of print).
Arnsten AFT, Wang M, Paspalas CD. Neuromodulation of thought: flexibilities and vulnerabilities in prefrontal cortical network synapses. Neuron. 2012;76:223–39.
Jin LE, Wang M, Galvin VC, Lightbourne TC, Conn PJ, Arnsten AFT, et al. mGluR2 vs. mGluR3 in primate prefrontal cortex: postsynaptic mGluR3 strengthen cognitive networks. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28:974–87.
Zhang Z, Bassam B, Thomas AG, Williams M, Liu J, Nance E, et al. Maternal inflammation leads to impaired glutamate homeostasis and up-regulation of glutamate carboxypeptidase II in activated microglia in the fetal/newborn rabbit brain. Neurobiol Dis. 2016;94:116–28.
Saini SM, Mancuso SG, Mostaid MS, Liu C, Pantelis C, Everall IP, et al. Meta-analysis supports GWAS-implicated link between GRM3 and schizophrenia risk. Transl Psychiatry. 2017;7:e1196.
Zink C, Barker P, Sawa A, Weinberger D, Wang A, Quillian H, et al. Missense mutation in FOLH1 is associated with decreased NAAG levels and impaired working memory circuitry and cognition. Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177:1129–39.
Arnsten AFT, Wang M. The evolutionary expansion of mGluR3-NAAG-GCPII signaling: relevance to human intelligence and cognitive disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177:1103–6.
Wang M, Ramos B, Paspalas C, Shu Y, Simen A, Duque A, et al. Alpha2A-adrenoceptor stimulation strengthens working memory networks by inhibiting cAMP-HCN channel signaling in prefrontal cortex. Cell. 2007;129:397–410.
Paspalas CD, Wang M, Arnsten AFT. Constellation of HCN Channels and cAMP regulating proteins in dendritic spines of the primate prefrontal cortex—potential substrate for working memory deficits in schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23:1643–54.
Carlyle BC, Nairn AC, Wang M, Yang Y, Jin LE, Simen AA, et al. cAMP-PKA phosphorylation of tau confers risk for degeneration in aging association cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:5036–41.
Datta D, Leslie SN, Wang M, Yang S-T, Morozov Y, Mentone S, et al. Age-related calcium dysregulation linked with tau pathology and impaired cognition in non-human primates. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17:920–32.
Havekes R, Park AJ, Tolentino RE, Bruinenberg VM, Tudor JC, Lee Y, et al. Compartmentalized PDE4A5 signaling impairs hippocampal synaptic plasticity and long-term memory. J Neurosci. 2016;36:8936–46.
MacKenzie KF, Wallace DA, Hill EV, Anthony DF, Henderson DJ, Houslay DM, et al. Phosphorylation of cAMP-specific PDE4A5 (phosphodiesterase-4A5) by MK2 (MAPKAPK2) attenuates its activation through protein kinase A phosphorylation. Biochem J. 2011;435:755–69.
Houslay KF, Christian F, MacLeod R, Adams DR, Houslay MD, Baillie GS. Identification of a multifunctional docking site on the catalytic unit of phosphodiesterase-4 (PDE4) that is utilised by multiple interaction partners. Biochem J. 2017;474:597–609.
Millar JK, Mackie S, Clapcote SJ, Murdoch H, Pickard BS, Christie S, et al. Disrupted in schizophrenia 1 and phosphodiesterase 4B: towards an understanding of psychiatric illness. J Physiol. 2007;584:401–5.
Moore TL, Schettler SP, Killiany RJ, Herndon JG, Luebke JI, Moss MB, et al. Cognitive impairment in aged rhesus monkeys associated with monoamine receptors in the prefrontal cortex. Behav Brain Res. 2005;160:208–21.
Colantuoni C, Hyde TM, Mitkus S, Joseph A, Sartorius L, Aguirre C, et al. Age-related changes in the expression of schizophrenia susceptibility genes in the human prefrontal cortex. Brain Struct Funct. 2008;213:255–71.
Wang M, Gamo NJ, Yang Y, Jin LE, Wang XJ, Laubach M, et al. Neuronal basis of age-related working memory decline. Nature. 2011;476:210–3.
Paspalas CD, Carlyle B, Leslie S, Preuss TM, Crimins JL, Huttner AJ, et al. The aged rhesus macaque manifests Braak-stage III/IV Alzheimer’s-like pathology. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2018;14:680–91.
Sawaguchi T, Goldman-Rakic PS. D1 dopamine receptors in prefrontal cortex: Involvement in working memory. Science. 1991;251:947–50.
Lidow MS, Goldman-Rakic PS, Gallager DW, Rakic P. Distribution of dopaminergic receptors in the primate cerebral cortex: quantitative autoradiographic analysis using [3H]raclopride, [3H]spiperone, and [3H]SCH 23390. Neurosci. 1991;40:657–71.
Smiley JF, Levey AI, Ciliax BJ, Goldman-Rakic PS. D1 dopamine receptor immunoreactivity in human and monkey cerebral cortex: predominant and extrasynaptic localization in dendritic spines. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1994;91:5720–4.
Wang M, Datta D, Enwright J, Galvin V, Yang S-T, Paspalas CD, et al. A novel dopamine D1 receptor agonist excites working memory-related neuronal firing in primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuropharmacol. 2019;150:46–58.
Arnsten AFT, Cai JX, Murphy BL, Goldman-Rakic PS. Dopamine D1 receptor mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged monkeys. Psychopharmacology. 1994;116:143–51.
Murphy BL, Arnsten AFT, Goldman-Rakic PS, Roth RH. Increased dopamine turnover in the prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance in rats and monkeys. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. 1996;93:1325–9.
Zahrt J, Taylor JR, Mathew RG, Arnsten AFT. Supranormal stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance. J Neurosci. 1997;17:8528–35.
Cai JX, Arnsten AFT. Dose-dependent effects of the dopamine D1 receptor agonists A77636 or SKF81297 on spatial working memory in aged monkeys. J Pharm Exp Ther. 1997;283:183–9.
Williams GV, Goldman-Rakic PS. Modulation of memory fields by dopamine D1 receptors enhances memory fields of prefrontal cortex. Nature. 1995;376:572–75.
Vijayraghavan S, Wang M, Birnbaum SG, Bruce CJ, Williams GV, Arnsten AFT. Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:376–84.
Arnsten AFT, Goldman-Rakic PS. Noise stress impairs prefrontal cortical cognitive function in monkeys: evidence for a hyperdopaminergic mechanism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:362–69.
Vijayraghavan S, Major AJ, Everling S. Dopamine D1 and D2 receptors make dissociable contributions to dorsolateral prefrontal cortical regulation of rule-guided oculomotor behavior. Cell Rep. 2016;16:805–16.
Vijayraghavan S, Major AJ, Everling S. Neuromodulation of prefrontal cortex in non-human primates by dopaminergic receptors during rule-guided flexible behavior and cognitive control. Front Neural Circuits. 2017;11:91.
Cools R, D’Esposito M. Inverted-U shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:e113–25.
Furman DJ, Zhang Z, Chatham CH, Good M, Badre D, Hsu M, et al. Augmenting frontal dopamine tone enhances maintenance over gating processes in working memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2020:1–13.
Furman DJ, White RL, Naskolnakorn J, Ye J, Kayser A, D’Esposito M. Effects of dopaminergic drugs on cognitive control processes vary by genotype. J Cogn Neurosci. 2020;32:804–21.
Fallon SJ, van der Schaaf ME, Ter Huurne N, Cools R. The neurocognitive cost of enhancing cognition with methylphenidate: improved distractor resistance but impaired updating. J Cogn Neurosci. 2017;29:652–63.
Jacob SN, Stalter M, Nieder A. Cell-type-specific modulation of targets and distractors by dopamine D1 receptors in primate prefrontal cortex. Nat Commun. 2016;7:13218.
Gamo NJ, Lur G, Higley MJ, Wang M, Paspalas CD, Vijayraghavan S, et al. Stress impairs prefrontal cortical function via D1 dopamine receptor interactions with HCN channels. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78:860–70.
Wang M, Vijayraghavan S, Goldman-Rakic PS. Selective D2 receptor actions on the functional circuitry of working memory. Science. 2004;303:853–56.
Ott T, Nieder A. Dopamine and cognitive control in prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019;23:213–34.
Ott T, Jacob SN, Nieder A. Dopamine receptors differentially enhance rule coding in primate prefrontal cortex neurons. Neuron. 2014;84:1317–28.
Ott T, Nieder A. Dopamine D2 receptors enhance population dynamics in primate prefrontal working memory circuits. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27:4423–35.
Coldwell MC, Boyfield I, Brown AM, Stemp G, Middlemiss DN. Pharmacological characterization of extracellular acidification rate responses in human D2(long), D3 and D4.4 receptors expressed in Chinese hamster ovary cells. Br J Pharm. 1999;127:1135–44.
Mrzljak L, Bergson C, Pappy M, Levenson R, Huff R, Goldman-Rakic PS. Localization of dopamine D4 receptors in GABAergic neurons of the primate brain. Nature. 1996;381:245–8.
Mueller A, Krock RM, Shepard S, Moore T. Dopamine receptor expression among local and visual cortex-projecting frontal eye field neurons. Cereb Cortex. 2020;30:148–64.
Noudoost B, Moore T. Control of visual cortical signals by prefrontal dopamine. Nature. 2011;474:372–5.
Van Tol HHM, Bunzow JR, Guan H-C, Sunahara RK, Seeman P, Niznik HB, et al. Cloning of the gene for a human dopamine D4 receptor with high affinity for the antipsychotic clozapine. Nature. 1991;350:610–4.
Arnsten AFT. Through the looking glass: differential noradrenergic modulation of prefrontal cortical function. Neural Plasticity. 2000;7:133–46.
Li B-M, Mei Z-T. Delayed response deficit induced by local injection of the alpha-2 adrenergic antagonist yohimbine into the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in young adult monkeys. Behav Neural Biol. 1994;62:134–9.
Birnbaum SG, Gobeske KT, Auerbach J, Taylor JR, Arnsten AFT. A role for norepinephrine in stress-induced cognitive deficits: alpha-1-adrenoceptor mediation in prefrontal cortex. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:1266–74.
Arnsten AFT, Goldman-Rakic PS. Alpha-2 adrenergic mechanisms in prefrontal cortex associated with cognitive decline in aged nonhuman primates. Science. 1985;230:1273–6.
Cai JX, Ma Y, Xu L, Hu X. Reserpine impairs spatial working memory performance in monkeys: reversal by the alpha-2 adrenergic agonist clonidine. Brain Res. 1993;614:191–6.
Li B-M, Mao Z-M, Wang M, Mei Z-T. Alpha-2 adrenergic modulation of prefrontal cortical neuronal activity related to spatial working memory in monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacol. 1999;21:601–10.
Arnsten AFT, Cai JX, Goldman-Rakic PS. The alpha-2 adrenergic agonist guanfacine improves memory in aged monkeys without sedative or hypotensive side effects. J Neurosci. 1988;8:4287–98.
Mao Z-M, Arnsten AFT, Li B-M. Local infusion of alpha-1 adrenergic agonist into the prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance in monkeys. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:1259–65.
Rama P, Linnankoski I, Tanila H, Pertovaara A, Carlson S. Medetomidine, atipamezole, and guanfacine in delayed response performance of aged monkeys. Pharm Biochem Behav. 1996;54:1–7.
Swartz BE, Kovalik E, Thomas K, Torgersen D, Mandelkern MA. The effects of an alpha-2 adrenergic agonist, guanfacine, on rCBF in human cortex in normal controls and subjects with focal epilepsy. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;23:263–75.
Arnsten AFT, Contant TA. Alpha-2 adrenergic agonists decrease distractability in aged monkeys performing a delayed response task. Psychopharmacology. 1992;108:159–69.
Wang M, Tang ZX, Li BM. Enhanced visuomotor associative learning following stimulation of alpha 2A-adrenoceptors in the ventral prefrontal cortex in monkeys. Brain Res. 2004;1024:176–82.
Kim S, Bobeica I, Gamo NJ, Arnsten AF, Lee D. Effects of α-2A adrenergic receptor agonist on time and risk preference in primates. Psychopharmacology. 2012;219:363–75.
Steere JC, Arnsten AFT. The alpha-2A noradrenergic agonist, guanfacine, improves visual object discrimination reversal performance in rhesus monkeys. Behav Neurosci. 1997;111:1–9.
Arnsten AFT. Guanfacine’s mechanism of action in treating prefrontal cortical disorders: successful translation across species. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2020;176:107327.
Birnbaum SB, Yuan P, Wang M, Vijayraghavan S, Bloom A, Davis D, et al. Protein kinase C overactivity impairs prefrontal cortical regulation of working memory. Science. 2004;306:882–84.
Datta D, Yang ST, Galvin VC, Solder J, Luo F, Morozov YM, et al. Noradrenergic α1-adrenoceptor actions in the primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2019;39:2722–34.
Bouret S, Sara SJ. Network reset: a simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline function. Trends Neurosci. 2005;28:574–82.
Arnsten AFT, Raskind M, Taylor FB, Connor DF. The effects of stress exposure on prefrontal cortex: translating basic research into successful treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder. Neurobiol Stress. 2015;1:89–99.
Ramos B, Colgan L, Nou E, Ovadia S, Wilson SR, Arnsten AFT. The beta-1 adrenergic antagonist, betaxolol, improves working memory performance in rats and monkeys. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58:894–900.
Green A, Ellis KA, Ellis J, Bartholomeusz CF, Ilic S, Croft RJ, et al. Muscarinic and nicotinic receptor modulation of object and spatial n-back working memory in humans. Pharm Biochem Behav. 2005;81:575–84.
Bloemendaal M, van Schouwenburg MR, Miyakawa A, Aarts E, D’Esposito M, Cools R. Dopaminergic modulation of distracter-resistance and prefrontal delay period signal. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232:1061–70.
Cools R, Sheridan M, Jacobs E, D’Esposito M. Impulsive personality predicts dopamine-dependent changes in frontostriatal activity during component processes of working memory. J Neurosci. 2007;27:5506–14.
Gibbs SE, D’Esposito M. A functional MRI study of the effects of bromocriptine, a dopamine receptor agonist, on component processes of working memory. Psychopharmacology. 2005;180:644–53.
Gibbs SE, D’Esposito M. Individual capacity differences predict working memory performance and prefrontal activity following dopamine receptor stimulation. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2005;5:212–21.
Gibbs SE, D’Esposito M. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the effects of pergolide, a dopamine receptor agonist, on component processes of working memory. Neuroscience. 2006;139:359–71.
Kimberg DY, D’Esposito M, Farah MJ. Effects of bromocriptine on human subjects depend on working memory capacity. Neuroreport. 1997;8:3581–85.
Kimberg DY, D’Esposito M. Cognitive effects of the dopamine receptor agonist pergolide. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:1020–07.
Luciana M, Depue RA, Arbisi P, Leon A. Facililtation of working memory in humans by a D2 dopamine receptor agonist. J Cogn Neurosci. 1992;4:58–68.
Luciana M, Collins PF. Dopaminergic modulation of working memory for spatial but not object cues in normal volunteers. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997;9:330–47.
Luciana L, Collins PF, Depue RA. Opposing roles for dopamine and serotonin in the modulation of human spatial working memory functions. Cereb Cortex. 1998;8:218–26.
Mehta MA, Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Mavaddat N, Pickard JD, Robbins TW. Methylphenidate enhances working memory by modulating discrete frontal and parietal lobe regions in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2000;20:RC651–56.
Mehta MA, McGowan SW, Lawrence AD, Aitken MR, Montgomery AJ, Grasby PM. Systemic sulpiride modulates striatal blood flow: relationships to spatial working memory and planning. NeuroImage. 2003;20:1982–94.
Mehta MA, Goodyer IM, Sahakian BJ. Methylphenidate improves working memory and set-shifting in AD/HD: relationships to baseline memory capacity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45:293–305.
Muller U, von Cramon DY, Pollman S. D1- versus D2-receptor modulation of visuospatial working memory in humans. J Neurosci. 1998;18:2720–28.
Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, Matthews K, Bannerjea A, Rimmer J, Robbins TW. Effects of methylphenidate on spatial working memory and planning in healthy young adults. Psychopharmacology. 1997;131:196–206.
Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Hodges JR, Summers BA, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Dopamine-dependent frontostriatal planning deficits in early Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology. 1995;9:126–40.
van der Schaaf ME, van Schouwenburg MR, Geurts DEM, Schellekens AFA, Buitelaar JK, Verkes RJ, et al. Establishing the dopamine dependency of human striatal signals during reward and punishment reversal learning. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:633–42.
van Holstein M, Aarts E, van der Schaaf ME, Geurts DE, Verkes RJ, Franke B, et al. Human cognitive flexibility depends on dopamine D2 receptor signaling. Psychopharmacol (Berl). 2011;218:567–78.
Soutschek A, Kozak R, de Martinis N, Howe W, Burke CJ, Fehr E, et al. Activation of D1 receptors affects human reactivity and flexibility to valued cues. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45:780–5.
Roffman JL, Tanner AS, Eryilmaz H, Rodriguez-Thompson A, Silverstein NJ, Ho NF, et al. Dopamine D1 signaling organizes network dynamics underlying working memory. Sci Adv. 2016;2:e1501672.
Abi-Dargham A, Mawlawi O, Lombardo I, Gil R, Martinez D, Huang Y, et al. Prefrontal dopamine D1 receptors and working memory in schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2002;22:3708–19.
Abi-Dargham A, Xu X, Thompson JL, Gil R, Kegeles LS, Urban NB, et al. Increased prefrontal cortical D1 receptors in drug naive patients with schizophrenia: a PET study with [11C]NNC112. J Psychopharmacol. 2012;26:794–805.
MacDonald SW, Karlsson S, Rieckmann A, Nyberg L, Bäckman L. Aging-related increases in behavioral variability: relations to losses of dopamine D1 receptors. J Neurosci. 2012;32:8186–91.
Duan H, Wearne SL, Rocher AB, Macedo A, Morrison JH, Hof PR. Age-related dendritic and spine changes in corticocortically projecting neurons in macaque monkeys. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:950–61.
Rosell DR, Zaluda LC, McClure MM, Perez-Rodriguez MM, Strike KS, Barch DM, et al. Effects of the D1 dopamine receptor agonist dihydrexidine (DAR-0100A) on working memory in schizotypal personality disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:446–53.
Floresco SB. Prefrontal dopamine and behavioral flexibility: shifting from an “inverted-U” toward a family of functions. Front Neurosci. 2013;7:62.
Seamans JK, Yang CR. The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Prog Neurobiol. 2004;74:1–58.
Durstewitz D, Seamans J. The dual-state theory of prefrontal cortex dopamine function with relevance to catechol-o-methyltransferase genotypes and schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatr. 2008;64:739–49.
Cools R, Barker RA, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:1136–43.
Frank MJ, Seeberger LC, O’Reilly RC. By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science. 2004;306:1940–3.
Mehta MA, Manes FF, Magnolfi G, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Impaired set-shifting and dissociable effects on tests of spatial working memory following the dopamine D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride in human volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2004;176:331–42.
Cools R, Sheridan M, Jacobs E, D’Esposito M. Impulsive personality predicts dopamine-dependent changes in frontostriatal activity during component processes of working memory. J Neurosci. 2007;27:5506–14.
Cools R, Frank MJ, Gibbs SE, Miyakawa A, Jagust W, D’Esposito M. Striatal dopamine predicts outcome-specific reversal learning and its sensitivity to dopaminergic drug administration. J Neurosci. 2009;29:1538–43.
Wallace DL, Vytlacil JJ, Nomura EM, Gibbs SEB, D’Esposito M. The dopamine agonist bromocriptine differentially affects fronto-striatal functional connectivity during working memory. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;5:32.
Mehta M, Calloway P, Sahakian B. Amelioration of specific working memory deficits by methylphenidate in a case of adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Psychopharm. 2000;14:299–302.
Frank MJ, O’Reilly RC. A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: psychopharmacological studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. Behav Neurosci. 2006;120:497–517.
Swart JC, Froböse MI, Cook JL, Geurts DE, Frank MJ, Cools R, et al. Catecholaminergic challenge uncovers distinct Pavlovian and instrumental mechanisms of motivated (in)action. eLife. 2017;6:e22169.
Hofmans L, Papadopetraki D, van den Bosch R, Määttä JI, Froböse MI, Zandbelt BB, et al. Methylphenidate boosts choices of mental labor over leisure depending on striatal dopamine synthesis capacity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45:2170–9.
Westbrook A, van den Bosch R, Määttä JI, Hofmans L, Papadopetraki D, Cools R, et al. Dopamine promotes cognitive effort by biasing the benefits versus costs of cognitive work. Science. 2020;367:1362–6.
Mattay VS, Goldberg TE, Fera F, Hariri AR, Tessitore A, Egan MF, et al. Catechol O-methyltransferase Val158 -met genotype and individual variation in the brain response to amphetamine. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:6186–91.
Aarts E, Roelofs A, Frank B, Rijpkema M, Fernandez G, Helmich RC, et al. Striatal dopamine mediates the interface between motivational and cognitive control in humans: evidence from genetic imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1943–51.
Cohen MX, Krohn-Grimberghe A, Elger CE, Weber B. Dopamine gene predicts the brain’s response to dopaminergic drug. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;26:3652–60.
Kayser AS, Mitchell JM, Weinstein D, Frank MJ. Dopamine, locus of control, and the exploration-exploitation tradeoff. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:454–62.
Cools R, Stefanova E, Barker RA, Robbins TW, Owen AM. Dopaminergic modulation of high-level cognition in Parkinson’s disease: the role of the prefrontal cortex revealed by PET. Brain. 2002;125:584–94.
Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Callicott JH, Bertonlino A, Goldberg TE, Chase TN, et al. Dopaminergic modulation of cortical function in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol. 2002;58:630–35.
Cools R, Gibbs SE, Miyakawa A, Jagust W, D’Esposito M. Working memory capacity predicts dopamine synthesis capacity in the human striatum. J Neurosci. 2008;28:1208–12.
Landau SM, Lal R, O’Neil JP, Baker S, Jagust WJ. Striatal dopamine and working memory. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:445–54.
Richfield EK, Penney JB, Young AB. Anatomical and affinity state comparisons between dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in the rat central nervous system. Neuroscience. 1989;30:767–77.
Froudist-Walsh S, Bliss DP, Ding X, Jankovic-Rapan L, Niu M, Knoblauch K, et al. A dopamine gradient controls access to distributed working memory in monkey cortex. bioRxiv 2020;2020.09.07.286500.
Smiley JF, Williams SM, Szigeti K, Goldman-Rakic PS. Light and electron microscopic characterization of dopamine-immunoreactive processes in human cerebral cortex. J Comp Neurol. 1992;321:325–35.
Choi EY, Drayna GK, Badre D. Evidence for a functional hierarchy of association networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2018;30:722–36.
Badre D, Nee DE. Frontal cortex and the hierarchical control of behavior. Trends Cogn Sci. 2018;22:170–88.
Pycock CJ, Carter CJ, Kerwin RW. Effect of 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex on neurotransmitter systems in subcortical sites in the rat. J Neurochem. 1980;34:91–9.
Pycock CJ, Kerwin RW, Carter CJ. Effect of lesion of cortical dopamine terminals on subcortical dopamine receptors in rats. Nature. 1980;286:74–7.
Karreman M, Moghaddam B. The prefrontal cortex regulates the basal release of dopamine in the limbic striatum: an effect mediated by ventral tegmental area. J Neurochemistry. 1996;66:589–98.
Takahashi YK, Stalnaker TA, Roesch MR, Schoenbaum G. Effects of inference on dopaminergic prediction errors depend on orbitofrontal processing. Behav Neurosci. 2017;131:127–34.
Starkweather CK, Gershman SJ, Uchida N. The medial prefrontal cortex shapes dopamine reward prediction errors under state uncertainty. Neuron. 2018;98:616–29.
Daw ND, Gershman SJ, Seymour B, Dayan P, Dolan RJ. Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron. 2011;69:1204–15.
Babayan BM, Uchida N, Gershman SJ. Belief state representation in the dopamine system. Nat Commun. 2018;9:1891.
Dabney W, Kurth-Nelson Z, Uchida N, Starkweather CK, Hassabis D, Munos R, et al. A distributional code for value in dopamine-based reinforcement learning. Nature. 2020;577:671–5.
Strafella AP, Paus T, Barrett J, Dagher A. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human prefrontal cortex induces dopamine release in the caudate nucleus. J Neurosci. 2001;21:RC157.
Strafella AP, Paus T, Fraraccio M, Dagher A. Striatal dopamine release induced by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human motor cortex. Brain. 2003;126:2609–15. Pt 12
Strafella AP, Ko JH, Grant J, Fraraccio M, Monchi O. Corticostriatal functional interactions in Parkinson’s disease: a rTMS/[11C]raclopride PET study. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22:2946–52.
Ko JH, Monchi O, Ptito A, Bloomfield P, Houle S, Strafella AP. Theta burst stimulation-induced inhibition of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex reveals hemispheric asymmetry in striatal dopamine release during a set-shifting task: a TMS-[(11)C]raclopride PET study. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28:2147–55.
Chamberlain SR, Müller U, Blackwell AD, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Noradrenergic modulation of working memory and emotional memory in humans. Psychopharmacology. 2006;188:397–407.
Iwanami A, Saito K, Fujiwara M, Okutsu D, Ichikawa H. Efficacy and safety of guanfacine extended-release in the treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: results of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2020;81:19m12979.
Connor DF, Findling RL, Kollins SH, Sallee F, López FA, Lyne A, et al. Effects of guanfacine extended release on oppositional symptoms in children aged 6-12 years with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and oppositional symptoms: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. CNS Drugs. 2010;24:755–68.
Jiang S, Czuma R, Cohen-Oram A, Hartney K, Stern TA. Guanfacine for hyperactive delirium: a case series. J Acad Consult Liaison Psychiatry. 2021;62:83–8.
O’Neill J, Fitten LJ, Siembieda DW, Ortiz F, Halgren E. Effects of guanfacine on three forms of distraction in the aging macaque. Life Sci. 2000;67:877–85.
Ota T, Yamamuro K, Okazaki K, Kishimoto T. Evaluating guanfacine hydrochloride in the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adult patients: design, development and place in therapy. Drug Des Devel Ther. 2021;15:1965–9.
Hernaus D, Casales Santa MM, Offermann JS, Van Amelsvoort T. Noradrenaline transporter blockade increases fronto-parietal functional connectivity relevant for working memory. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017;27:399–410.
Bymaster FP, Katner JS, Nelson DL, Hemrick-Luecke SK, Threlkeld PG, Heiligenstein JH, et al. Atomoxetine increases extracellular levels of norepinephrine and dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rat: a potential mechanism for efficacy in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:699–711.
Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2005;28:403–50.
Arnsten AF, Paspalas CD, Gamo NJ, Yang Y, Wang M. Dynamic network connectivity: a new form of neuroplasticity. Trends Cog Sci. 2010;14:365–75. PMCID: PMC2914830
Shine JM, van den Brink RL, Hernaus D, Nieuwenhuis S, Poldrack RA. Catecholaminergic manipulation alters dynamic network topology across cognitive states. Netw Neurosci. 2018;2:381–96.
Hermans EJ, van Marle HJ, Ossewaarde L, Henckens MJ, Qin S, van Kesteren MT, et al. Stress-related noradrenergic activity prompts large-scale neural network reconfiguration. Science. 2011;334:1151–3.
Hermans EJ, Henckens MJ, Joëls M, Fernández G. Dynamic adaptation of large-scale brain networks in response to acute stressors. Trends Neurosci. 2014;37:304–14.
Dayan P, Yu AJ. Phasic norepinephrine: a neural interrupt signal for unexpected events. Network. 2006;17:335–50.
Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Blackwell AD, Clark L, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Neurochemical modulation of response inhibition and probabilistic learning in humans. Science. 2006;311:861–3.
Borchert RJ, Rittman T, Passamonti L, Ye Z, Sami S, Jones SP, et al. Atomoxetine enhances connectivity of prefrontal networks in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:2188.
O’Callaghan C, Hezemans FH, Ye R, Rua C, Jones PS, Murley AG, et al. Locus coeruleus integrity and the effect of atomoxetine on response inhibition in Parkinson’s disease. medRxiv. 2020:2020.09.03.20176800.
Rae CL, Nombela C, Rodríguez PV, Ye Z, Hughes LE, Jones PS, et al. Atomoxetine restores the response inhibition network in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2016;139:2235–48. Pt 8
Ye Z, Altena E, Nombela C, Housden CR, Maxwell H, Rittman T, et al. Improving response inhibition in Parkinson’s disease with atomoxetine. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:740–8.
Freeman ZT, Rice KA, Soto PL, Pate KA, Weed MR, Ator NA, et al. Neurocognitive dysfunction and pharmacological intervention using guanfacine in a rhesus macaque model of self-injurious behavior. Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5:e567.
Fox H, Sofuoglu M, Sinha R. Guanfacine enhances inhibitory control and attentional shifting in early abstinent cocaine-dependent individuals. J Psychopharmacol. 2015;29:312–23.
Arnsten AF, Pliszka SR. Catecholamine influences on prefrontal cortical function: relevance to treatment of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and related disorders. Pharm Biochem Behav. 2011;99:211–6.
Muller TH, Mars RB, Behrens TE, O’Reilly JX. Control of entropy in neural models of environmental state. Elife. 2019;8:e39404.
Lawson RP, Bisby J, Nord CL, Burgess N, Rees G. The computational, pharmacological, and physiological determinants of sensory learning under uncertainty. Curr Biol. 2021;31:163–72.e4.
Qin S, Cousijn H, Rijpkema M, Luo J, Franke B, Hermans EJ, et al. The effect of moderate acute psychological stress on working memory-related neural activity is modulated by a genetic variation in catecholaminergic function in humans. Front Integr Neurosci. 2012;6:16.
Dubois M, Habicht J, Michely J, Moran R, Dolan R, Hauser TU. Human complex exploration strategies are enriched by noradrenaline-modulated heuristics. eLife. 2021;10:e59907.
Hauser TU, Moutoussis M, Purg N, Dayan P, Dolan RJ. Beta-blocker propranolol modulates decision urgency during sequential information gathering. J Neurosci. 2018;38:7170–78.
Warren CM, Wilson RC, van der Wee NJ, Giltay EJ, van Noorden MS, Cohen JD, et al. The effect of atomoxetine on random and directed exploration in humans. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0176034.
Frank MJ, Doll BB, Oas-Terpstra J, Moreno F. Prefrontal and striatal dopaminergic genes predict individual differences in exploration and exploitation. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:1062–68.
Bentley P, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Cholinergic modulation of cognition: insights from human pharmacological functional neuroimaging. Prog Neurobiol. 2011;94:360–88.
Silver MA, Shenhav A, D’Esposito M. Cholinergic enhancement reduces spatial spread of visual responses in human early visual cortex. Neuron. 2008;60:904–14.
Coppola JJ, Disney AA. Most calbindin-immunoreactive neurons, but few calretinin-immunoreactive neurons, express the m1 acetylcholine receptor in the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. Brain Behav. 2018;8:e01071.
Gratton C, Yousef S, Aarts E, Wallace DL, D’Esposito M, Silver MA. Cholinergic, but not dopaminergic or noradrenergic, enhancement sharpens visual spatial perception in humans. J Neurosci. 2017;37:4405–15.
Sun Y, Yang Y, Galvin VC, Yang S, Arnsten AF, Wang M. Nicotinic α4β2 cholinergic receptor influences on dorsolateral prefrontal cortical neuronal firing during a working memory task. J Neurosci. 2017;37:5366–77.
Mocking RJ, Patrick Pflanz C, Pringle A, Parsons E, McTavish SF, Cowen PJ, et al. Effects of short-term varenicline administration on emotional and cognitive processing in healthy, non-smoking adults: a randomized, double-blind, study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:476–84.
Loughead J, Ray R, Wileyto EP, Ruparel K, Sanborn P, Siegel SJ, et al. Effects of the alpha4beta2 partial agonist varenicline on brain activity and working memory in abstinent smokers. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:715–21.
Bentley P, Husain M, Dolan RJ. Effects of cholinergic enhancement on visual stimulation, spatial attention, and spatial working memory. Neuron. 2004;41:969–82.
Hasselmo ME, Sarter M. Modes and models of forebrain cholinergic neuromodulation of cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:52–73.
Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FA, Rafal RD. How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? Neuropsychologia. 1987;25:135–45.
Voytko ML, Olton DS, Richardson RT, Gorman LK, Tobin JT, Price DL. Basal forebrain lesions in monkeys disrupt attention but not learning and memory. J Neurosci. 1994;14:167–86.
Yu AJ, Dayan P. Uncertainty, neuromodulation, and attention. Neuron 2005;46:681–92.
Luciana M, Burgund ED, Berman MG, Hanson KL. Effects of tryptophan loading on verbal, spatial and affective working memory functions in healthy adults. J Psychopharmacol. 2001;15:219–30.
Cano-Colino M, Almeida R, Gomez-Cabrero D, Artigas F, Compte A. Serotonin regulates performance nonmonotonically in a spatial working memory network. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:2449–63.
Evers EA, Cools R, Clark L, van der Veen FM, Jolles J, Sahakian BJ, et al. Serotonergic modulation of prefrontal cortex during negative feedback in probabilistic reversal learning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:1138–47.
den Ouden HE, Daw ND, Fernandez G, Elshout JA, Rijpkema M, Hoogman M, et al. Dissociable effects of dopamine and serotonin on reversal learning. Neuron. 2013;80:1090–100.
Rogers RD, Blackshaw AJ, Middleton HC, Matthews K, Hawtin K, Crowley C, et al. Tryptophan depletion impairs stimulus-reward learning while methylphenidate disrupts attentional control in healthy young adults: implications for the monoaminergic basis of impulsive behaviour. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146:482–91.
Talbot PS, Watson DR, Barrett SL, Cooper SJ. Rapid tryptophan depletion improves decision-making cognition in healthy humans without affecting reversal learning or set shifting. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:1519–25.
Meyniel F, Goodwin GM, Deakin JFW, Klinge C, MacFadyen C, Milligan H, et al. A specific role for serotonin in overcoming effort cost. eLife. 2016;5:e17282.
Crockett MJ, Cools R. Serotonin and aversive processing in affective and social decision-making. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015;5:64–70.
Cools R, Nakamura K, Daw ND. Serotonin and dopamine: unifying affective, activational, and decision functions. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:98–113.
Schweighofer N, Bertin M, Shishida K, Okamoto Y, Tanaka SC, Yamawaki S, et al. Low-serotonin levels increase delayed reward discounting in humans. J Neurosci. 2008;28:4528–32.
Boureau Y-L, Dayan P. Opponency revisited: competition and cooperation between dopamine and serotonin. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:74–97.
Campbell-Meiklejohn D, Wakeley J, Herbert V, Cook J, Scollo P, Ray MK, et al. Serotonin and dopamine play complementary roles in gambling to recover losses. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:402–10.
Crockett MJ, Clark L, Robbins TW. Reconciling the role of serotonin in behavioral inhibition and aversion: acute tryptophan depletion abolishes punishment-induced inhibition in humans. J Neurosci. 2009;29:11993–9.
Geurts DEM, Huys QJM, den Ouden HEM, Cools R. Serotonin and aversive Pavlovian control of instrumental behavior in humans. J Neurosci. 2013;33:18932–39.
Dayan P, Huys QJM. Serotonin in affective control. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:95–126.
Clark L, Roiser JP, Cools R, Rubinsztein DC, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Stop signal response inhibition is not modulated by tryptophan depletion or the serotonin transporter polymorphism in healthy volunteers: implications for the 5-HT theory of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology. 2005;182:570–8.
Cools R, Blackwell A, Clark L, Menzies L, Cox S, Robbins T. Tryptophan depletion disrupts the motivational guidance of goal-directed behavior as a function of trait impulsivity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:1362–73.
Bari A, Theobald DE, Caprioli D, Mar AC, Aidoo-Micah A, Dalley JW, et al. Serotonin modulates sensitivity to reward and negative feedback in a probabilistic reversal learning task in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1290–301.
Eagle DM, Lehmann O, Theobald DE, Pena Y, Zakaria R, Ghosh R, et al. Serotonin depletion impairs waiting but not stop-signal reaction time in rats: implications for theories of the role of 5-HT in behavioral inhibition. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:1311–21.
Rubia K, Lee F, Cleare AJ, Tunstall N, Fu CH, Brammer M, et al. Tryptophan depletion reduces right inferior prefrontal activation during response inhibition in fast, event-related fMRI. Psychopharmacology 2005;179:791–803.
Evers EA, van der Veen FM, van Deursen JA, Schmitt JA, Deutz NE, Jolles J. The effect of acute tryptophan depletion on the BOLD response during performance monitoring and response inhibition in healthy male volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2006;187:200–8.
LeMarquand DG, Benkelfat C, Pihl RO, Palmour RM, Young SN. Behavioral disinhibition induced by tryptophan depletion in nonalcoholic young men with multigenerational family histories of paternal alcoholism. Am J Psychiatr. 1999;156:1771–9.
Hebart MN, Gläscher J. Serotonin and dopamine differentially affect appetitive and aversive general Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232:437–51.
Ren J, Friedmann D, Xiong J, Liu CD, Ferguson BR, Weerakkody T, et al. Anatomically defined and functionally distinct dorsal raphe serotonin sub-systems. Cell. 2018;175:472–87.e20.
Seo C, Guru A, Jin M, Ito B, Sleezer BJ, Ho YY, et al. Intense threat switches dorsal raphe serotonin neurons to a paradoxical operational mode. Science. 2019;363:538–42.
Amat J, Baratta MV, Paul E, Bland ST, Watkins LR, Maier SF. Medial prefrontal cortex determines how stressor controllability affects behavior and dorsal raphe nucleus. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:365–71.
Doya K. Metalearning and neuromodulation. Neural Netw. 2002;15:495–506.
Dayan P. Twenty-five lessons from computational neuromodulation. Neuron. 2012;76:240–56.
Reyes G, Silva JR, Jaramillo K, Rehbein L, Sackur J. Self-knowledge dim-out: stress impairs metacognitive accuracy. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0132320.
Acknowledgements
We thank the editors for the opportunity to write this review.
Funding
AFTA is supported by NIH R01 AG061190-01 and a Neuronex NSF grant NSF 2015276. RC is supported by NWO Vici 453-14-005 and an Ammodo Science (KNAW) award. AFTA and Yale University receive royalties from the USA sales of Intuniv (extended release guanfacine) from Shire/Takeda Pharmaceuticals, but do not receive royalties from generic or nonUSA sales. AFTA also serves as a consultant to Lundbeck, Supernus, and Vallon Pharma regarding potential mechanisms for cognitive restoration. RC is consultant for Roche Pharma Research, but does not receive financial compensation.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
RC and AFTA contributed equally to the conceptualization and writing of this review.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Cools, R., Arnsten, A.F.T. Neuromodulation of prefrontal cortex cognitive function in primates: the powerful roles of monoamines and acetylcholine. Neuropsychopharmacol. 47, 309–328 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01100-8
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01100-8
This article is cited by
-
Are there links between Alzheimer’s disease and ADHD? The efficacy of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors and NMDA receptor antagonists in controlling ADHD symptoms: a systematic review
Middle East Current Psychiatry (2024)
-
Medial prefrontal cortex acetylcholine signaling mediates the ability to learn an active avoidance response following learned helplessness training
Neuropsychopharmacology (2024)
-
Microglia regulate sleep through calcium-dependent modulation of norepinephrine transmission
Nature Neuroscience (2024)
-
Timescales of learning in prefrontal cortex
Nature Reviews Neuroscience (2024)
-
Rhythmicity of neuronal oscillations delineates their cortical and spectral architecture
Communications Biology (2024)