Key Points
-
Novel or unexpected events or stimuli draw our attention and are more easily remembered than predictable or familiar ones. Evidence from a variety of sources is beginning to clarify the neural mechanisms that contribute to this response to novelty.
-
When a stimulus is repeated, the neuronal response to it (in many parts of the brain) often decreases rapidly. The response is highest when the stimulus is novel. Behavioural responses also depend on stimulus novelty: the initial presentation seems to produce 'priming' so that repeated stimuli are processed more efficiently. Repetition suppression of neuronal responses occurs over short timescales and is stimulus-specific. Stimulus repetition can also cause suppression of responses over longer time periods, including over weeks of training on a task.
-
This effect seems to rely on synaptic plasticity mediated by NMDA (N-methyl-D-aspartate) receptors. It has been proposed that the reduction in population activity represents a sparsening of the response to a stimulus that leaves a more robust response carried by fewer neurons. Reductions in responses in early visual areas might result from decreased feedback from higher visual areas of cortex.
-
Another type of novelty that has been studied is contextual novelty. This arises when an unexpected stimulus or event occurs, for example a dog's bark amidst a string of birdsong. Stimuli that are contextually novel produce characteristic event-related potentials in human electroencephalograms. These potentials seem to be generated by frontal and temporal areas of cortex. Evidence from functional imaging studies supports the idea that a network of brain areas responds to contextual novelty. This network probably produces the enhanced memory that is seen for contextually novel stimuli.
-
The neurotransmitter acetylcholine and the neuromodulator noradrenaline seem to be important for the generation of this response. An integrated view of novelty processing could see the release of acetylcholine and noradrenaline being increased in response to a novel stimulus, and modulating the activity of the cortical areas to which they project. This in turn would lead to the production of novelty-related potentials and the modulation of synaptic plasticity.
Abstract
The ability to detect and respond to novel events is crucial for survival in a rapidly changing environment. Four decades of neuroscientific research has begun to delineate the neural mechanisms by which the brain detects and responds to novelty. Here, we review this research and suggest how changes in neural processing at the cellular, synaptic and network levels allow us to detect, attend to and subsequently remember the occurrence of a novel event.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Subscription info for Japanese customers
We have a dedicated website for our Japanese customers. Please go to natureasia.com to subscribe to this journal.
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Tulving, E. & Schacter, D. L. Priming and human memory systems. Science 247, 301–306 (1990).
Gilbert, C. D., Sigman, M. & Crist, R. E. The neural basis of perceptual learning. Neuron 31, 681–697 (2001).
Ringo, J. L. Stimulus specific adaptation in inferior temporal and medial temporal cortex of the monkey. Behav. Brain Res. 76, 191–197 (1996).
Henson, R. N. & Rugg, M. D. Neural response suppression, haemodynamic repetition effects, and behavioural priming. Neuropsychologia 41, 263–270 (2003).
Habib, R. On the relation between conceptual priming, neural priming, and novelty assessment. Scand. J. Psychol. 42, 187–195 (2001).
Miller, E. K., Erickson, C. A. & Desimone, R. Neural mechanisms of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex of the macaque. J. Neurosci. 16, 5154–5167 (1996).
Rainer, G., Rao, S. C. & Miller, E. K. Prospective coding for objects in primate prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 19, 5493–5505 (1999).
Riches, I. P., Wilson, F. A. & Brown, M. W. The effects of visual stimulation and memory on neurons of the hippocampal formation and the neighboring parahippocampal gyrus and inferior temporal cortex of the primate. J. Neurosci. 11, 1763–1779 (1991).
Baylis, G. C. & Rolls, E. T. Responses of neurons in the inferior temporal cortex in short term and serial recognition memory tasks. Exp. Brain Res. 65, 614–622 (1987).
Sobotka, S. & Ringo, J. L. Stimulus specific adaptation in excited but not in inhibited cells in inferotemporal cortex of macaque. Brain Res. 646, 95–99 (1994).
Miller, E. K., Gochin, P. M. & Gross, C. G. Habituation-like decrease in the responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex of the macaque. Vis. Neurosci. 7, 357–362 (1991).
Miller, E. K. & Desimone, R. Parallel neuronal mechanisms for short-term memory. Science 263, 520–522 (1994).
Maffei, L., Fiorentini, A. & Bisti, S. Neural correlate of perceptual adaptation to gratings. Science 182, 1036–1038 (1973).
Movshon, J. A. & Lennie, P. Pattern-selective adaptation in visual cortical neurones. Nature 278, 850–852 (1979).
Dragoi, V., Sharma, J. & Sur, M. Adaptation-induced plasticity of orientation tuning in adult visual cortex. Neuron 28, 287–298 (2000).
Carandini, M., Barlow, H. B., O'Keefe, L. P., Poirson, A. B. & Movshon, J. A. Adaptation to contingencies in macaque primary visual cortex. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 352, 1149–1154 (1997).
Sanchez-Vives, M. V., Nowak, L. G. & McCormick, D. A. Cellular mechanisms of long-lasting adaptation in visual cortical neurons in vitro. J. Neurosci. 20, 4286–4299 (2000).
Sohal, V. S. & Hasselmo, M. E. A model for experience-dependent changes in the responses of inferotemporal neurons. Network 11, 169–190 (2000). A modelling study that accounts for several key aspects of single-neuron data in the inferior temporal cortex during short-term memory tasks.
Brown, M. W. & Aggleton, J. P. Recognition memory: what are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2, 51–61 (2001).
Muller, J. R., Metha, A. B., Krauskopf, J. & Lennie, P. Rapid adaptation in visual cortex to the structure of images. Science 285, 1405–1408 (1999).
Li, L., Miller, E. K. & Desimone, R. The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 69, 1918–1929 (1993).
Rolls, E. T., Baylis, G. C., Hasselmo, M. E. & Nalwa, V. The effect of learning on the face selective responses of neurons in the cortex in the superior temporal sulcus of the monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 76, 153–164 (1989).
Fahy, F. L., Riches, I. P. & Brown, M. W. Neuronal activity related to visual recognition memory: long-term memory and the encoding of recency and familiarity information in the primate anterior and medial inferior temporal and rhinal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 96, 457–472 (1993).
Asaad, W. F., Rainer, G. & Miller, E. K. Neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex during associative learning. Neuron 21, 1399–1407 (1998).
Chen, L. L. & Wise, S. P. Supplementary eye field contrasted with the frontal eye field during acquisition of conditional oculomotor associations. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 1122–1134 (1995).
Schoups, A., Vogels, R., Qian, N. & Orban, G. Practising orientation identification improves orientation coding in V1 neurons. Nature 412, 549–553 (2001).
Ghose, G. M., Yang, T. & Maunsell, J. H. Physiological correlates of perceptual learning in monkey V1 and V2. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 1867–1888 (2002).
Brown, M. W. & Xiang, J. Z. Recognition memory: neuronal substrates of the judgement of prior occurrence. Prog. Neurobiol. 55, 149–189 (1998).
Kourtzi, Z. & Kanwisher, N. Cortical regions involved in perceiving object shape. J. Neurosci. 20, 3310–3318. (2000).
Buckner, R. L. et al. Functional-anatomic correlates of object priming in humans revealed by rapid presentation event-related fMRI. Neuron 20, 285–296 (1998).
Ranganath, C., Johnson, M. K. & D'Esposito, M. Left anterior prefrontal activation increases with demands to recall specific perceptual information. J. Neurosci. 20, RC108 (2000).
Ranganath, C. & D'Esposito, M. Medial temporal lobe activity associated with active maintenance of novel information. Neuron 31, 865–873 (2001).
Grill-Spector, K. & Malach, R. fMR-adaptation: a tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 107, 293–321 (2001).
Tolias, A. S., Smirnakis, S. M., Augath, M. A., Trinath, T. & Logothetis, N. K. Motion processing in the macaque: revisited with functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 21, 8594–8601 (2001).
Huk, A. C., Ress, D. & Heeger, D. J. Neuronal basis of the motion aftereffect reconsidered. Neuron 32, 161–172 (2001).
Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T. & Oeltermann, A. Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature 412, 150–157 (2001).
Logothetis, N. K. The neural basis of the blood-oxygen-level-dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging signal. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 357, 1003–1037 (2002).
Desimone, R. & Duncan, J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193–222 (1995).
Wiggs, C. L. & Martin, A. Properties and mechanisms of perceptual priming. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 8, 227–233 (1998).
Stark, C. E. & McClelland, J. L. Repetition priming of words, pseudowords, and nonwords. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 26, 945–972 (2000).
Grunwald, T. et al. Evidence relating human verbal memory to hippocampal N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 96, 12085–12089 (1999).
Rainer, G. & Miller, E. K. Timecourse of object-related neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex during a short-term memory task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15, 1244–1254 (2002).
Rainer, G. & Miller, E. K. Effects of visual experience on the representation of objects in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron 27, 179–189 (2000). A report demonstrating that reduced neural responses following stimulus repetition can reflect the development of a more sparse and robust object representation.
Sokolov, E. N. Higher nervous functions: the orienting reflex. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 25, 545–580 (1963).
Corbetta, M. & Shulman, G. L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215 (2002).
Sutton, S., Braren, M., Zubin, J. & John, E. R. Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science 150, 1187–1188 (1965).
Paller, K. A. in Cognitive Electrophysiology (eds Heinze, H. J., Munte, T. F. & Mangun, G. R.) 300–333 (Birkhauser, Boston, Massachusetts, 1994). A review paper summarizing studies of the P300 across different mammalian species.
Courchesne, E., Hillyard, S. A. & Galambos, R. Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 39, 131–143 (1975).
Squires, N. K., Squires, K. C. & Hillyard, S. A. Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 38, 387–401 (1975).
Halgren, E., Marinkovic, K. & Chauvel, P. Generators of the late cognitive potentials in auditory and visual oddball tasks. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 106, 156–164 (1998).
Friedman, D., Cycowicz, Y. M. & Gaeta, H. The novelty P3: an event-related brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain's evaluation of novelty. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 355–373 (2001). An extensive review describing the available findings on ERP correlates of contextual novelty, their neural sources, and the potential cognitive processes reflected by these ERPs.
Knight, R. T. & Nakada, T. Cortico-limbic circuits and novelty: a review of EEG and blood flow data. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 57–70 (1998).
Soltani, M. & Knight, R. T. Neural origins of the P300. Crit. Rev. Neurobiol. (in the press). A detailed review describing what is known about the neural sources that contribute to the novelty P3.
McCarthy, G. & Wood, C. C. Intracranial recordings of endogenous ERPs in humans. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 39 (suppl.), 331–337 (1987). | PubMed |
Johnson, R. Jr. On the neural generators of the P300 component of the event-related potential. Psychophysiology 30, 90–97 (1993).
Ruchkin, D. S., Johnson, R. Jr, Canoune, H. L., Ritter, W. & Hammer, M. Multiple sources of P3b associated with different types of information. Psychophysiology 27, 157–176 (1990).
Knight, R. T. Decreased response to novel stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 59, 9–20 (1984). The first in a series of investigations into the neural basis of the novelty P3.
Ranganath, C. & Paller, K. A. Frontal brain activity during episodic and semantic retrieval: insights from event-related potentials. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 598–609 (1999).
Cycowicz, Y. M. & Friedman, D. Effect of sound familiarity on the event-related potentials elicited by novel environmental sounds. 36, 30–51 (1998).
Friedman, D. & Simpson, G. V. ERP amplitude and scalp distribution to target and novel events: effects of temporal order in young, middle-aged and older adults. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2, 49–63 (1994).
Kazmerski, V. A. & Friedman, D. Repetition of novel stimuli in an ERP oddball paradigm: aging effects. J. Psychophysiol. 9, 298–311 (1995).
Knight, R. T. Contribution of human hippocampal region to novelty detection. Nature 383, 256–259 (1996).
Knight, R. T. Distributed cortical network for visual attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 75–91 (1997).
Simons, R. F., Graham, F. K., Miles, M. A. & Chen, X. On the relationship of P3a and the novelty-P3. Biol. Psychol. 56, 207–218 (2001).
Comerchero, M. D. & Polich, J. P3a, perceptual distinctiveness, and stimulus modality. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 7, 41–48 (1998).
Katayama, J. & Polich, J. Stimulus context determines P3a and P3b. Psychophysiology 35, 23–33 (1998).
Demiralp, T., Ademoglu, A., Comerchero, M. & Polich, J. Wavelet analysis of P3a and P3b. Brain Topogr. 13, 251–267 (2001).
Jeon, Y. W. & Polich, J. P3a from a passive visual stimulus task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 2202–2208 (2001).
Friedman, D., Kazmerski, V. A. & Cycowicz, Y. M. Effects of aging on the novelty P3 during attend and ignore oddball tasks. Psychophysiology 35, 508–520 (1998).
Clarke, J. M., Halgren, E. & Chauvel, P. Intracranial ERPs in humans during a lateralized visual oddball task: I. Occipital and peri-Rolandic recordings. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 1210–1225 (1999).
Clarke, J. M., Halgren, E. & Chauvel, P. Intracranial ERPs in humans during a lateralized visual oddball task: II. Temporal, parietal, and frontal recordings. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 1226–1244 (1999).
Halgren, E. et al. Endogenous potentials generated in the human hippocampal formation and amygdala by infrequent events. Science 210, 803–805 (1980).
Halgren, E. et al. Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distractor auditory and visual stimuli. I. Superior temporal plane and parietal lobe. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 94, 191–220 (1995).
Halgren, E. et al. Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distractor auditory and visual stimuli. II. Medial, lateral and posterior temporal lobe. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 94, 229–250 (1995).
Baudena, P., Halgren, E., Heit, G. & Clarke, J. M. Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distractor auditory and visual stimuli. III. Frontal cortex. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 94, 251–264 (1995).
Alain, C., Richer, F., Achim, A. & Saint Hilaire, J. M. Human intracerebral potentials associated with target, novel, and omitted auditory stimuli. Brain Topogr. 1, 237–245 (1989).
McCarthy, G., Wood, C. C., Williamson, P. D. & Spencer, D. D. Task-dependent field potentials in human hippocampal formation. J. Neurosci. 9, 4253–4268 (1989).
Grunwald, T. et al. Limbic P300s in temporal lobe epilepsy with and without Ammon's horn sclerosis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 1899–1906 (1999).
Daffner, K. R. et al. The central role of the prefrontal cortex in directing attention to novel events. Brain 123, 927–939 (2000).
Daffner, K. R. et al. Regulation of attention to novel stimuli by frontal lobes: an event-related potential study. Neuroreport 9, 787–791 (1998).
Yamaguchi, S. & Knight, R. T. Anterior and posterior association cortex contributions to the somatosensory P300. J. Neurosci. 11, 2039–2054 (1991).
Knight, R. T. & Scabini, D. Anatomic bases of event-related potentials and their relationship to novelty detection in humans. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 15, 3–13 (1998).
Knight, R. T., Scabini, D., Woods, D. L. & Clayworth, C. C. Contributions of temporal-parietal junction to the human auditory P3. Brain Res. 502, 109–116 (1989).
Luria, A. R. The Working Brain (Penguin, New York, 1966).
Daffner, K. R. et al. Disruption of attention to novel events after frontal lobe injury in humans. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 68, 18–24 (2000).
Rule, R. R., Shimamura, A. P. & Knight, R. T. Orbitofrontal cortex and dynamic filtering of emotional stimuli. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2, 264–270 (2002).
Kiehl, K. A. & Liddle, P. F. Reproducibility of the hemodynamic response to auditory oddball stimuli: a six-week test-retest study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 18, 42–52 (2003).
Kiehl, K. A., Laurens, K. R., Duty, T. L., Forster, B. B. & Liddle, P. F. Neural sources involved in auditory target detection and novelty processing: an event-related fMRI study. Psychophysiology 38, 133–142 (2001).
Kiehl, K. A., Laurens, K. R., Duty, T. L., Forster, B. B. & Liddle, P. F. An event-related fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks. J. Psychophysiol. 15, 221–240 (2001).
Clark, V. P., Fannon, S., Lai, S., Benson, R. & Bauer, L. Responses to rare visual target and distractor stimuli using event-related fMRI. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 3133–3139 (2000).
Kirino, E., Belger, A., Goldman-Rakic, P. & McCarthy, G. Prefrontal activation evoked by infrequent target and novel stimuli in a visual target detection task: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 20, 6612–6618 (2000).
Opitz, B., Mecklinger, A., Friederici, A. D. & von Cramon, D. Y. The functional neuroanatomy of novelty processing: integrating ERP and fMRI results. Cereb. Cortex 9, 379–391 (1999).
Downar, J., Crawley, A. P., Mikulis, D. J. & Davis, K. D. A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment. Nature Neurosci. 3, 277–283 (2000).
Huettel, S. A., Mack, P. B. & McCarthy, G. Perceiving patterns in random series: dynamic processing of sequence in prefrontal cortex. Nature Neurosci. 5, 485–490 (2002).
Strange, B. A. & Dolan, R. J. Adaptive anterior hippocampal responses to oddball stimuli. Hippocampus 11, 690–698 (2001).
Strange, B. A., Fletcher, P. C., Henson, R. N., Friston, K. J. & Dolan, R. J. Segregating the functions of human hippocampus. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 96, 4034–4039 (1999).
Poldrack, R. A. et al. Interactive memory systems in the human brain. Nature 414, 546–550 (2001).
Buchel, C., Dolan, R. J., Armony, J. L. & Friston, K. J. Amygdala-hippocampal involvement in human aversive trace conditioning revealed through event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 19, 10869–10876 (1999).
von Restorff, H. Uber die Wirkung von Bereichsbildungen im Spurenfeld. Psychol. Forsch. 18, 299–342 (1933).
Parker, A., Wilding, E. L. & Akerman, C. The von Restorff effect in visual object recognition memory in humans and monkeys: the role of frontal/perirhinal interaction. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 691–703 (1998). An experiment demonstrating that the processing advantage for contextually novel items, in an otherwise homogeneous list, is dependent on intact perirhinal–frontal interactions.
Hunt, R. R. The subtlety of distinctiveness: what von Restorff really did. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2, 105–112 (1995).
Wallace, W. P. Review of the historical, empirical, and theoretical status of the von Restorff phenomenon. Psychol. Bull. 63, 410–424 (1965).
Habib, R., McIntosh, A. R., Wheeler, M. A. & Tulving, E. Memory encoding and hippocampally-based novelty/familiarity discrimination networks. Neuropsychologia 41, 271–279 (2003).
Tulving, E., Markowitsch, H. J., Craik, F. E., Habib, R. & Houle, S. Novelty and familiarity activations in PET studies of memory encoding and retrieval. Cereb. Cortex 6, 71–79 (1996).
Fabiani, M. & Donchin, E. Encoding processes and memory organization: a model of the von Restorff effect. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 21, 224–240 (1995).
Fabiani, M., Karis, D. & Donc hin, E. Effects of mnemonic strategy manipulation in a von Restorff paradigm. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 75, 22–35 (1990).
Fabiani, M., Karis, D. & Donchin, E. P300 and recall in an incidental memory paradigm. Psychophysiology 23, 298–308 (1986).
Kirchhoff, B. A., Wagner, A. D., Maril, A. & Stern, C. E. Prefrontal–temporal circuitry for episodic encoding and subsequent memory. J. Neurosci. 20, 6173–6180 (2000).
Fernandez, G. & Tendolkar, I. Integrated brain activity in medial temporal and prefrontal areas predicts subsequent memory performance: human declarative memory formation at the system level. Brain Res. Bull. 55, 1–9 (2001).
Frodl-Bauch, T., Bottlender, R. & Hegerl, U. Neurochemical substrates and neuroanatomical generators of the event-related P300. Neuropsychobiology 40, 86–94 (1999).
Gu, Q. Neuromodulatory transmitter systems in the cortex and their role in cortical plasticity. Neuroscience 111, 815–835 (2002).
Mesulam, M. M. & Mufson, E. J. Neural inputs into the nucleus basalis of the substantia innominata (Ch4) in the rhesus monkey. Brain 107, 253–274 (1984). A careful characterization of the inputs to the NBM, revealing that, although this area modulates activity across several cortical regions, it receives input from only a few.
Mesulam, M. M., Volicer, L., Marquis, J. K., Mufson, E. J. & Green, R. C. Systematic regional differences in the cholinergic innervation of the primate cerebral cortex: distribution of enzyme activities and some behavioral implications. Ann. Neurol. 19, 144–151 (1986).
Easton, A. & Gaffan, D. Crossed unilateral lesions of the medial forebrain bundle and either inferior temporal or frontal cortex impair object-reward association learning in Rhesus monkeys. Neuropsychologia 39, 71–82 (2001). One of a series of recent lesion studies in monkeys by these authors that underscore the importance of the cholinergic system in learning and memory.
Easton, A., Parker, A. & Gaffan, D. Crossed unilateral lesions of medial forebrain bundle and either inferior temporal or frontal cortex impair object recognition memory in Rhesus monkeys. Behav. Brain Res. 121, 1–10 (2001).
Fine, A. et al. Learning impairments following injection of a selective cholinergic immunotoxin, ME20.4 IgG-saporin, into the basal nucleus of Meynert in monkeys. Neuroscience 81, 331–343 (1997). A study demonstrating that immunotoxic lesions specific to cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain result in cell loss in inferior temporal and frontal cortex of monkeys, the severity of which is correlated with behavioural impairments.
Hasselmo, M. E. Neuromodulation: acetylcholine and memory consolidation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 351–359 (1999).
Wilson, F. A. & Rolls, E. T. Neuronal responses related to the novelty and familiarity of visual stimuli in the substantia innominata, diagonal band of Broca and periventricular region of the primate basal forebrain. Exp. Brain Res. 80, 104–120 (1990).
Wilson, F. A. & Rolls, E. T. Learning and memory is reflected in the responses of reinforcement-related neurons in the primate basal forebrain. J. Neurosci. 10, 1254–1267 (1990).
Giovannini, M. G. et al. Effects of novelty and habituation on acetylcholine, GABA, and glutamate release from the frontal cortex and hippocampus of freely moving rats. Neuroscience 106, 43–53 (2001). A microdialysis study in behaving rats showing robust novelty-related changes in levels of ACh, but not glutamate, in rat frontal cortex.
Thiel, C. M., Henson, R. N. & Dolan, R. J. Scopolamine but not lorazepam modulates face repetition priming: a psychopharmacological fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 27, 282–292 (2002).
Thiel, C. M., Henson, R. N., Morris, J. S., Friston, K. J. & Dolan, R. J. Pharmacological modulation of behavioral and neuronal correlates of repetition priming. J. Neurosci. 21, 6846–6852 (2001).
Sperling, R. et al. Functional MRI detection of pharmacologically induced memory impairment. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 455–460 (2002).
Miller, E. K. & Desimone, R. Scopolamine affects short-term memory but not inferior temporal neurons. Neuroreport 4, 81–84 (1993).
Potter, D. D., Pickles, C. D., Roberts, R. C. & Rugg, M. D. The effect of cholinergic receptor blockade by scopolamine on memory performance and the auditory P3. J. Psychophysiol. 14, 11–23 (2000).
Potter, D. D., Pickles, C. D., Roberts, R. C. & Rugg, M. D. Scopolamine impairs memory performance and reduces frontal but not parietal visual P3 amplitude. Biol. Psychol. 52, 37–52 (2000).
Zaborszky, L., Cullinan, W. E. & Luine, V. N. Catecholaminergic–cholinergic interaction in the basal forebrain. Prog. Brain Res. 98, 31–49 (1993).
Morrison, J. H. & Foote, S. L. Noradrenergic and serotoninergic innervation of cortical, thalamic, and tectal visual structures in Old and New World monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 243, 117–138 (1986).
Aston-Jones, G., Chiang, C. & Alexinsky, T. Discharge of noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats and monkeys suggests a role in vigilance. Prog. Brain Res. 88, 501–520 (1991).
Foote, S. L., Berridge, C. W., Adams, L. M. & Pineda, J. A. Electrophysiological evidence for the involvement of the locus coeruleus in alerting, orienting, and attending. Prog. Brain Res. 88, 521–532 (1991).
Vankov, A., Herve-Minvielle, A. & Sara, S. J. Response to novelty and its rapid habituation in locus coeruleus neurons of the freely exploring rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 7, 1180–1187 (1995). A direct demonstration of novelty-related responses in single neurons of the locus coeruleus of freely behaving rats.
Pineda, J. A. & Westerfield, M. Monkey P3 in an 'oddball' paradigm: pharmacological support for multiple neural sources. Brain Res. Bull. 31, 689–696 (1993).
Guillem, F., Rougier, A. & Claverie, B. Short- and long-delay intracranial ERP repetition effects dissociate memory systems in the human brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 437–458 (1999).
Grunwald, T., Lehnertz, K., Heinze, H. J., Helmstaedter, C. & Elger, C. E. Verbal novelty detection within the human hippocampus proper. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 3193–3197 (1998).
Grunwald, T., Elger, C. E., Lehnertz, K., Van Roost, D. & Heinze, H. J. Alterations of intrahippocampal cognitive potentials in temporal lobe epilepsy. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 95, 53–62 (1995).
Elger, C. E. et al. Human temporal lobe potentials in verbal learning and memory processes. Neuropsychologia 35, 657–667 (1997).
McCarthy, G., Nobre, A. C., Bentin, S. & Spencer, D. D. Language-related field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: I. Intracranial distribution and neural generators. J. Neurosci. 15, 1080–1089 (1995).
Nobre, A. C. & McCarthy, G. Language-related field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: II. Effects of word type and semantic priming. J. Neurosci. 15, 1090–1098 (1995).
Loy, R., Koziell, D. A., Lindsey, J. D. & Moore, R. Y. Noradrenergic innervation of the adult rat hippocampal formation. J. Comp. Neurol. 189, 699–710 (1980).
Lewis, D. A. & Morrison, J. H. Noradrenergic innervation of monkey prefrontal cortex: a dopamine-β-hydroxylase immunohistochemical study. J. Comp. Neurol. 282, 317–330 (1989).
Acknowledgements
We thank C. Clayworth, R. Knight, K. Lamberty, K. Paller and M. Soltani for their generous assistance in figure preparation, and C. Brozinsky, N. Kroll and M. Kishiyama for helpful comments on earlier versions of this article. G.R. was supported by the Austrian Academy of Sciences and the Max Planck Society.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Related links
Related links
FURTHER INFORMATION
Encyclopedia of Life Sciences
brain imaging: localization of brain functions
Glossary
- DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE TASKS
-
Recognition memory tasks in which presentation of a stimulus is followed by a delay, after which a choice is offered. In matching tasks, the originally presented stimulus must be chosen; in non-matching tasks, a new stimulus must be selected. With small stimulus sets, the stimuli are frequently repeated and therefore become highly familiar. So, typically, such tasks are most readily solved by short-term or working memory rather than by long-term memory mechanisms.
- TILT AFTER-EFFECT
-
If you stare at a set of lines that are tilted in one direction from upright, upright lines will subsequently look as though they are tilted in the opposite direction.
- TEMPORAL DECORRELATION
-
Small eye movements made during free viewing of natural scenes tend to expose neurons to similar but not identical structure. Adaptation can reduce responses to structure that is similar across fixations, removing correlations.
- EYE FIELD
-
An area that receives visual inputs and produces movements of the eye.
- CONDITIONAL OCULOMOTOR LEARNING
-
An association between a set of stimuli and a set of eye movements has to be learned by trial and error, where each stimulus is associated with a particular eye movement.
- EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS
-
Electrical potentials that are generated in the brain as a consequence of the synchronized activation of neuronal networks by external stimuli. These evoked potentials are recorded at the scalp and consist of precisely timed sequences of waves or 'components'.
- EVENT-RELATED fMRI
-
A variant of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) methods that allows neural correlates of individual trials or classes of trials to be isolated and compared.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Ranganath, C., Rainer, G. Neural mechanisms for detecting and remembering novel events. Nat Rev Neurosci 4, 193–202 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1052
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1052
This article is cited by
-
Changes in statistical learning across development
Nature Reviews Psychology (2023)
-
Long-term, multi-event surprise correlates with enhanced autobiographical memory
Nature Human Behaviour (2023)
-
Fear of the new? Geckos hesitate to attack novel prey, feed near objects and enter a novel space
Animal Cognition (2023)
-
Novelty detection and orienting: effects on skin conductance and heart rate
Psychological Research (2023)
-
A neural theory for counting memories
Nature Communications (2022)