Abstract
We examined whether the comprehension of narrative texts differed between auditory, audiovisual, and written text presentations in a sample of 8- and 10-year-olds and adults. Based on multi-level theories of text comprehension that assume text comprehension to involve at least three levels of mental representation, we applied a sentence recognition task that enabled the separate assessment of the memory of the text surface, text base, and the situation model. Results indicate that 8-year-olds benefit from audiovisual and auditory text presentations in comparison with written text presentations in terms of their memory of situation model information. For 10-year-olds and adults, their text comprehension did not differ between audiovisual, auditory, and written text presentations. Additionally, the mode of text presentation had no effect on the memory of text surface and text base information.
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.References
Artelt C et al. (2007) Förderung von Lesekompetenz—Expertise (BMBF ed.). BMBF, Bonn
Barsalou LW (1999) Language comprehension: Archival memory or preparation for situated action? Discourse Processes 28(1):61–80
Barsalou LW (2008) Grounded cognition. Annu Rev Psychol 59:617–645
Bäuerlein K (2014) Leseverständnisdiagnostik in der Sekundarstufe-Theoretische Grundlagen sowie Konstruktion und empirische Erprobung der Lesetests LESEN 6-7 und LESEN 8-9. Würzburg University Press, Würzburg
Beagles-Roos J, Gat I (1983) Specific impact of radio and television on children’s story comprehension. J Educ Psychol 75(1):128
Beentjes JW, Van Der Voort TH (1991) Children’s written accounts of televised and printed stories. Educ Technol Res Dev 39(3):15–26
Beentjes JW, Vooijs MW, Van Der Voort TH (1993) Children’s recall of televised and printed news as a function of test expectation. J Educ Televis 19(1):5–13
Brunyé TT et al (2010) You heard it here first: readers mentally simulate described sounds. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 135(2):209–215
Byrne M, Curtis R (2000) Designing health communication: testing the explanations for the impact of communication medium on effectiveness. Br J Health Psychol 5(2):189–199
Carney RN, Levin JR (2002) Pictorial illustrations still improve students’ learning from text. Educ Psychol Rev 14(1):5–26
Christie B, Collyer J (2008) Do video clips add more value than audio clips? Presenting industrial research and development results using multimedia. Behav Inf Technol 27(5):395–405
Chun DM, Plass JL (1997) Research on text comprehension in multimedia environments. Lang Learn Technol 1(1):60–81
Clement CA, Gentner D (1991) Systematicity as a selection constraint in analogical mapping. Cogn Sci 15(1):89–132
Corston R, Colman AM (1997) Modality of communication and recall of health-related information. J Health Psychol 2(2):185–194
Danks JH, End LJ (1987) Processing strategies for reading and listening. In: Horowitz R, Samuels SJ (eds) Comprehending oral and written language. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 271–294
DeFleur ML et al (1992) Audience recall of news stories presented by newspaper, computer, television and radio. Journal Mass Commun Q 69(4):1010–1022
Diergarten AK, Nieding G (2015) Children’s and adults’ ability to build online emotional inferences during comprehension of audiovisual and auditory texts. J Cogn Dev 16(2):381–406
Diergarten AK, Nieding G (2016) Online emotional inferences in written and auditory texts: a study with children and adults. Read Writ 29(7):1383–1407. doi:10.1007/s11145-016-9642-z
Engelen JA et al (2011) Perceptual simulation in developing language comprehension. J Exp Child Psychol 110(4):659–675
Faul F et al (2007) G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 39(2):175–191
Field A (2013) Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. Sage, London
Fletcher CR, Chrysler ST (1990) Surface forms, textbases, and situation models: recognition memory for three types of textual information. Discourse Processes 13(2):175–190
Fletcher JD, Tobias S (2005) Cognitive theory of multimedia learning. In: Mayer RE (ed) The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 117–134
Florit E, Cain K (2011) The simple view of reading: is it valid for different types of alphabetic orthographies? Educ Psychol Rev 23(4):553–576
Furnham A, Gunter B (1985) Sex, presentation mode and memory for violent and non-violent news. J Educ Televis 11(2):99–105
Furnham A, Gunter B (1987) Effects of time of day and medium of presentation on immediate recall of violent and non-violent news. Appl Cogn Psychol 1(4):255–262
Furnham A, Williams C (1987) Remembering commercials presented in different media. J Educ Televis 13(2):115–124
Furnham A, Benson I, Gunter B (1989) Memory for television commercials as a function of the channel of communication. Soc Behav 3:105–112
Gentner D (1983) Structure-mapping: a theoretical framework for analogy. Cogn Sci 7(2):155–170
Gernsbacher M (1990) Language comprehension as structure building. Erlbaum, Hillsdale
Gibbons J et al (1986) Young children’s recall and reconstruction of audio and audiovisual narratives. Child Dev 57(4):1014–1023
Glenberg AM, Kaschak MP (2002) Grounding language in action. Psychon Bull Rev 9(3):558–565
Gough PB, Tunmer WE (1986) Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and special education 7(1):6–10
Gough PB, Hoover WA, Peterson CL (1996) Some observations on a simple view of reading. In: Cornoldi C, Oakhill J (eds) Reading comprehension difficulties: processes and intervention. Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 1–13
Greenhoot AF, Semb PA (2008) Do illustrations enhance preschoolers’ memories for stories? Age-related change in the picture facilitation effect. J Exp Child Psychol 99(4):271–287
Gunter B, Furnham A, Griffiths S (2000) Children’s memory for news: a comparison of three presentation media. Media Psychol 2(2):93–118
Guttmann J, Levin JR, Pressley M (1977) Pictures, partial pictures, and young children’s oral prose learning. J Educ Psychol 69(5):473–480
Hayes DS, Kelly SB, Mandel M (1986) Media differences in children’s story synopses: radio and television contrasted. J Educ Psychol 78(5):341
Hemforth B, Konieczny L (2008) Saetze und Texte verstehen und produzieren. In: Muesseler J, Prinz W (eds) Lehrbuch Allgemeine Psychologie. Spektrum, Heidelberg, pp 504–552
Hildyard A, Olson DR (1978) Memory and inference in the comprehension of oral and written discourse. Discourse Processes 1(2):91–117
Hoover WA, Gough PB (1990) The simple view of reading. Read Writ 2(2):127–160
Horton WS, Rapp DN (2003) Out of sight, out of mind: occlusion and the accessibility of information in narrative comprehension. Psychon Bull Rev 10(1):104–110
Isberner M-B et al (2013) Comprehending conflicting science-related texts: graphs as plausibility cues. Instr Sci 41(5):849–872
Johnson-Laird PN (1983) Mental models: towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Harvard University Press, Cambridge
Kalyuga S et al (2003) The expertise reversal effect. Educ Psychol 38(1):23–31
Kendeou P et al (2005) Developing successful readers: building early comprehension skills through television viewing and listening. Early Childhood Educ J 33(2):91–98
Kendeou P et al (2008) Children’s inference generation across different media. J Res Read 31(3):259–272
Kintsch W (1988) The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: a construction-integration model. Psychol Rev 95(2):163
Kintsch W (1998) Comprehension: a paradigm for cognition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Kürschner C, Schnotz W, Eid M (2006) Konstruktion mentaler Repräsentationen beim Hör-und Leseverstehen. Z Medienpsychol 18(2):48–59
MacWhinney B (1999) The emergence of language from embodiment. In: MacWhinney B (ed) The emergence of language. Lawrence Erlabaum, Mahwah, pp 213–256
Maier J, Richter T (2013) How nonexperts understand conflicting information on social science issues: the role of perceived plausibility and reading goals. J Media Psychol Theories Methods Appl 25(1):14
Marx H, Jungmann T (2000) Abhängigkeit der Entwicklung des Leseverstehens von Hörverstehen und grundlegenden Lesefertigkeiten im Grundschulalter: eine Prüfung des Simple View of reading-Ansatzes. Z für Entwicklungspsychol Pädagog Psychol 32(2):81–93
Mayer RE (1997) Multimedia learning: are we asking the right questions? Educ Psychol 32(1):1–19
Mayer RE (2005) Principles for reducing extraneous processing in multimedia learning: coherence, signaling, redundancy, spatial contiguity, and temporal contiguity principles. In: Mayer RE (ed) The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 183–200
Mayer RE, Heiser J, Lonn S (2001) Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: when presenting more material results in less understanding. J Educ Psychol 93(1):187
McKoon G, Ratcliff R (1980) Priming in item recognition: the organization of propositions in memory for text. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 19(4):369–386
Narvaez D et al (2011) Are older adults more attuned to morally charged information? Exp Aging Res 37(4):398–434
Nieding G (2006) Wie verstehen Kinder Texte? Die Entwicklung kognitiver Repräsentationen [How do children understand texts? The development of cognitive representations]. Pabst Science Publ, Lengerich
Paivio A (1986) Mental representations. A dual coding approach. Oxford University Press, New York
Peirce JW (2007) PsychoPy—psychophysics software in Python. J Neurosci Methods 162(1):8–13
Peirce JW (2009) Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. In: Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, vol 2, p 10. http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/neuro.11.010.2008
Pezdek K, Lehrer A, Simon S (1984) The relationship between reading and cognitive processing of television and radio. Child Dev 55(6):2072–2082
Pollack I, Norman DA (1964) A non-parametric analysis of recognition experiments. Psychon Sci 1(1–12):125–126
Radvansky GA et al (2003) Aging and situation model updating. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 10(2):158–166
Reder LM (1982) Plausibility judgments versus fact retrieval: alternative strategies for sentence verification. Psychol Rev 89(3):250
Ricci CM, Beal CR (2002) The effect of interactive media on children’s story memory. J Educ Psychol 94(1):138
Richter T, Christmann U (2002) Lesekompetenz: Prozessebenen und interindividuelle Unterschiede. In: Groeben N, Hurrelmann B (eds) Lesekompetenz: Bedingungen, Dimensionen, Funktionen. Juventa, Weinheim, pp 25–58
Richter T, Zwaan RA (2009) Processing of color words activates color representations. Cognition 111(3):383–389
Rideout V (2013) Zero to eight: children’s media use in America. Common Sense Media, San Francisco. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/file/zero-to-eight-2013pdf-0/download
Rosebrock C, Nix D (2011) Grundlagen der Lesedidaktik und der systematischen schulischen Leseförderung. Schneider-Verlag, Hohengehren
Sachs JS (1974) Memory in reading and listening to discourse. Mem Cogn 2(1):95–100
Salomon G (1979) Media and symbol systems as related to cognition and learning. J Educ Psychol 71(2):131–148
Schmalhofer F, Glavanov D (1986) Three components of understanding a programmer’s manual: verbatim, propositional, and situational representations. J Mem Lang 25(3):279–294
Schnotz W (1993) On the relation of dual coding and mental models in graphics comprehension. Learning and Instruction 3(3):247–249
Schnotz W (2014) Integrated model of text and picture comprehension. In: Mayer R (ed) The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 72–103
Schnotz W, Bannert M (2003) Construction and interference in learning from multiple representation. Learning and instruction 13(2):141–156
Schroeder S et al (2015) childLex: a lexical database of German read by children. Behav Res Methods 47(4):1085–1094
Spearritt D (1962) Listening comprehension: A factorial analysis. Australian Council for Education Research, Melbourne
Stanfield RA, Zwaan RA (2001) The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychol Sci 12(2):153–156
Stanislaw H, Todorov N (1999) Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 31(1):137–149
Stanovich KE (1991) Discrepancy definitions of reading disability: Has intelligence led us astray? Read Res Q 26(1):7–29
Stanovich KE, Siegel LS (1994) Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: a regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. J Educ Psychol 86(1):24
Stanovich KE, West RF (1989) Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Read Res Q 24(4):402–433
Sticht TG, James JH (1984) Listening and reading. In: Pearson PD et al (eds) Handbook of reading research. Longman, New York, pp 293–317
Sweller J (2005) The redundancy principle in multimedia learning. In: Mayer RE (ed) The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 159–168
Therriault DJ, Yaxley RH, Zwaan RA (2009) The role of color diagnosticity in object recognition and representation. Cogn Process 10(4):335–342
Tibus M, Heier A, Schwan S (2013) Do films make you learn? Inference processes in expository film comprehension. J Educ Psychol 105(2):329
Trabasso T, Van Den Broek P (1985) Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events. J Mem Lang 24(5):612–630
Unsöld I, Nieding G (2009) Die Bildung prädiktiver Inferenzen von Kindern und Erwachsenen bei der kognitiven Verarbeitung audiovisueller und auditiver Texte [Predictive inferences in children and adults during the cognitive processing of audiovisual and auditory texts]. Z Entwicklungspsychol Pädagog Psychol 41(2):87–95
Van Dijk TA, Kintsch W (1983) Strategies of discourse comprehension. Academic Press, New York
Vosniadou S, Ortony A (1989) Similarity and analogical reasoning. Cambridge University Press, New York
Walma Van Der Molen JH, Van Der Voort THA (1997) Children’s recall of television and print news: a media comparison study. J Educ Psychol 89(1):82
Walma Van Der Molen JH, Van Der Voort THA (2000) The impact of television, print, and audio on children’s recall of the news. Hum Commun Res 26(1):3–26
Wannagat W et al (2016) Mental representations of the text surface, the text base, and the situation model in auditory and audiovisual texts in 7-, 9-, and 11-year-olds. Discourse Process. doi:10.1080/0163853X.2016.1237246
Wicks RH, Drew DG (1991) Learning from news: effects of message consistency and medium on recall and inference making. Journal Mass Commun Q 68(1–2):155–164
Zwaan RA (2004) The immersed experiencer: toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In: Ross B (ed) Psychology of learning and motivation. Academic Press, New York, pp 35–62
Zwaan RA (2014) Embodiment and language comprehension: reframing the discussion. Trends Cogn Sci 18(5):229–234
Zwaan RA, Radvansky GA (1998) Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychol Bull 123(2):162
Zwaan RA, Singer M (2003) Text comprehension. In: Graesser AC, Gernsbacher MA, Goldman SR (eds) Handbook of discourse processes. Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 83–121
Acknowledgements
This research was funded by a grant of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) awarded to Gerhild Nieding (Ni496/9-1). We wish to thank the participating children, parents, and teachers for their participation and cooperation. We also thank our research assistants and master students for their help with the data collection. Frank Maier supervised the recording of the stories, and Pauline Feichtinger drew the pictures. Thank you!
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Handling editor: Ekaterina Shutova (University of Cambridge); Reviewers: Rolf Zwaan (Erasmus Universiteit Rotterdam), Jan Engelen (Tilburg University).
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Wannagat, W., Waizenegger, G. & Nieding, G. Multi-level mental representations of written, auditory, and audiovisual text in children and adults. Cogn Process 18, 491–504 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-017-0820-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-017-0820-y