Abstract
This study determined the spatial resolution of the virtual surface profile created by ultrasound haptic stimulation. We assumed the case where a finger moves along the surface of a virtual object. This object was produced by low-frequency lateral modulation that creates a pseudo static force. We defined the spatial resolution as the minimum distance required to discriminate between two virtual bumps. Several sensory channels are combined when a human feels the geometric features of a surface. This paper focuses on mesoscopic shapes, whose representative length ranges from a few millimeters to fingertip size. We considered two strategies to present mesoscopic shapes: changing either the contact position or the force strength. We measured the spatial resolutions in mesoscopic shapes created by each method, and discussed which factor is more effective to perceive mesoscopic features.
Supported in part by JST CREST JPMJCR18A2.
You have full access to this open access chapter, Download conference paper PDF
Similar content being viewed by others
Keywords
1 Introduction
Among noncontact tactile presentation techniques using air jets [1], vortex rings [2], lasers [3], and ultrasound [4,5,6], the technique using ultrasound can reproduce several tactile sensations, including geometric features of surfaces. In this study, we assume the case where a finger moves along the surface of a virtual object and discuss the spatial resolution as the basic parameter. The base of this research is the pseudo-pressure presentation [7] recently demonstrated by Morisaki et al. Since static pressure sensation is generated by low-frequency lateral modulation (LM) [8], reproducing spatial unevenness by changing the presented pressure in synchronization with the finger movement is possible.
Multiple sensory signals are used for a human to perceive geometric features of a surface haptically. For example, when capturing a macroscopic shape whose curvature radius is larger than the finger size, the curvature is determined by synthesizing the trajectory and surface angle of the perceived finger using proprioceptors and cutaneous receptors, respectively, during the finger motion [9]. Meanwhile, high-frequency vibration induced by the hand’s stroking motion is the main factor when sensing the microscopic features of the surface. A fine surface structure whose representative length is smaller than the depth of the mechanical receptors in the skin or the spacing between the adjacent fingerprint ridges can induce overall vibration perceived using deep mechanoreceptors [10, 11].
This paper focuses on mesoscopic shapes, whose representative length ranges from a few millimeters to fingertip size. These shapes can be perceived using superficial mechanoreceptors as the spatial pattern. Here, we distinguish between these mesoscopic and microscopic sensations though the word roughness encompasses both. In noncontact tactile displays, it remains unclear to what extent ultrasound can present detailed mesoscopic shapes, since it has a relatively large wavelength of 8.5 mm in 40 kHz in the air, which is the typical frequency of the current midair haptics.
In related studies, Matsubayashi et al. [12, 13] proposed a method for displaying object shapes in a finger pad. However, these studies were limited to cases where the virtual objects vertically contact the skin. The surface roughness has already been considered in midair haptics studies [14, 15]. Nevertheless, since static pressure sensation cannot be reproduced, presenting static shapes is difficult except for some special situations [16].
This study considers two basic strategies for presenting mesoscopic shapes. One is the contact position change (CPC) method, in which we change the center of the contact area between the surface shape and fingertip synchronously with the fingertip position. The other is the contact strength change (CSC) method, in which we change the sum of the forces using the fingertip position. This CSC method is similar to the approach proposed by Howard et al. [17], however, both strategies focus on different scales. The CSC method focuses on mesoscopic shapes, whereas Howard et al.’s research focused on macroscopic shapes.
In real contact, both the position and intensity of the contact change, nonetheless, we intentionally present them separately to clarify the spatial resolution in both strategies.
2 Methods
2.1 Overview of the Methods
Figure 1 shows the overview of the CPC and CSC methods. In the following experiments, the trajectory of the finger is on the horizontal plane, with no vertical motion. However, we assume that the finger moves on the uneven rigid body surface and estimate the contact point and contact force. In the calculation, we assume that the finger is a circular rigid body.
The CPC method, shown in Fig. 1(a), reproduces the surface irregularity by presenting the contact point estimated from the finger position, keeping the contact force constant. It is unclear whether the tactile stimuli reproduced using the CPC method expresses unevenness. However, since the tactile stimulus reproduced by the CPC method was perceived as “soft and easily deformable convex objects” in the preliminary experiment, it was considered a stimulus to express unevenness. When the skin traces the surface of a rigid object, the contact pressure changes according to the unevenness. Additionally, the height of the finger changes up and down. However, when the stimulus is presented by the CPC method, the presenting pressure is assumed to be constant and the trajectory of the finger is along the horizontal plane. In such a situation, the presented target surface is perceived as a very soft and easily deformable object.
Meanwhile, Fig. 1(b) shows a method for calculating the contact pressure using the CSC method. Here, the contact pressure is given proportional to the height of the contact point. In the case of contact with a real object, both the contact position and pressure change. In other words, by using CPC and CSC together appropriately, can reproduce slight irregularities on the surface of a rigid body. In this research, our scope excludes tactile reproduction, but we will examine how fine the unevenness pattern can be expressed by changing the position and intensity of the 40 kHz ultrasonic focus.
2.2 Contact Position Change Method
The focus position in the CPC method is given as follows. Assume the finger center is at \(x = x_f\) and the ultrasound focus position is expressed as \((x, y) = (x_p, y_p)\). As shown in Fig. 1(a), \((x_p, y_p)\) is obtained
from geometric considerations, where the radii of curvature of the finger and virtual object are \(r_f\) and \(r_c\), respectively, and the x-axis center of the virtual convex surface is \(x_c\). Here, we assume \(r_f = 7\) mm. Moreover, we set \(r_c\) at 5, 15, 25, and 35 mm, respectively, to investigate the virtual convex surfaces.
Two adjacent convex surfaces are presented in Fig. 1(c). The contact point is calculated as follows: First, we determine the centers of the circles \((x_1, y_c)\) and \((x_2, y_c)\), circle radius \(r_c\), and height h from the base plane. In this study, we fixed \(h = 5\) mm throughout the experiments. Also, two peaks are symmetrically arranged around \(x = 0\).
If the finger position \(x_f\) is negative or positive, it is considered the finger contacts the left or right circles, respectively. The contact point is only one and calculated as explained above, although, in real time, the finger might touch multiple points on the surface. The tactile stimuli reproduced using the CPC method in the preliminary experiment were perceived as two bumps.
2.3 Contact Strength Change Method
The CSC method is shown in Fig. 1(b). We assumed that the total contact force P is proportional to the height of the contact point, which is given as
where \(P_M\) is the Max Pressure, the maximum force strength of the device, and
where \(\theta \) is the angle indicating the contact point between the finger and virtual object, while \(\theta _1\) corresponds to the minimal force strength when the finger touches the plane.
In the CSC method, the calculated pressure is applied to the finger pad center. Similar to the CPC method, the same parameters were used in the CSC-method experiments as \(r_f = 7\) mm, \(h =\) 5 mm, and \(r_c\) at 5, 15, 25, and 35 mm, respectively.
2.4 Ultrasound Focus Point Presentation Method
In previous explanations, we simply used the term “contact pressure.” In midair ultrasound tactile presentation, however, one problem is that the stimulus is too weak to perceive when a time-constant radiation pressure is applied to a fixed point on the skin. To avoid this problem, we used a low-frequency LM so that the stimulus is perceived even when the finger is stationary.
Recent studies have shown that LM modulation at low frequencies 10 Hz produces pseudo-pressure sensation [7]. Thus, the finger can feel pressure sensations with distinct spatial localization. In this study, we used the linear movement LM as shown in Fig. 1(d). The focus is oscillated sinusoidally at f [Hz] along the finger at \(z=l\; \mathrm {sin}(2\pi f t)\), where z is the focus position along the finger. We set \(l =\) 1.2 cm and \(f = 15\) [Hz]. The number of points per cycle in the LM vibration was set to 50 to avoid unnecessary vibration.
3 Experiment
To clarify the threshold for discriminating convex surfaces, we developed a system and conducted experiments as follows.
3.1 System
Figure 2(a) shows the presentation system consisting of an infrared touch screen (ITS), GreenTouch, GT-IRTK156-1, 15.6 in., and an airborne ultrasound phased array (AUPA) [18].
ITS is a position sensor device that locates the two-dimensional position coordinates of a finger (or something else) using infrared transmitting and receiving tubes densely placed on the four sides of the screen. For this system, it was used to obtain the position coordinate in the moving direction of the finger (i.e., the x-axis direction in Fig. 2(a)). Meanwhile, AUPA is a device that creates an ultrasound focal point at an arbitrary three-dimensional position by controlling the amplitude and phase of each ultrasound transducer arranged in an array. Here, six AUPAs were used to form the focal point using the proposed methods described in Sect. 2.
The AUPA and ITS were synchronized by using virtual reality development environment (Unity 2020.3.13f1, produced by Unity Technologies) to put the ultrasound focus on the finger using the finger coordinates captured by the ITS. In addition, users were required to put their fingers on a linear rail to maintain a constant finger height.
3.2 Procedure
The following procedure was performed in the CPC and CSC methods. The procedure follows the declaration of Helsinki (2013), and participants provided written informed consent before the study.
First, before starting the experiment, we visually displayed the two convex surfaces with the video made by Unity. In the video, we prepared the assumed convex surfaces and the supposed finger was linked to the actual finger. Next, participants were asked to answer whether they felt two convex surfaces as one or two according to displayed curvatures and distances. During the experiment, participants listened to white noise to exclude the influence of the driving noise of AUPAs, and judged the convex number using their haptics without the video. In this convex surface presentation, the sensation was presented on the convex surfaces, and not on the plane surface. Also, the exploring speed of the finger was unspecified.
In the experiment, we prepared two convex surfaces with the equal heights and radii (\(h=5\) mm, \(r_c\) at 5, 15, 25, 35 mm, respectively), and obtained the discrimination threshold of the two convex surfaces using the 1-up, 1-down staircase method for each radius. In the beginning, the distance between the centers of both convex surfaces (henceforth, distance d, see Fig. 1(c)) was set to 0 mm. We asked participants to answer whether they felt one convex surface or two convex surfaces within 3-laps exploration. We changed the distance d by 2 mm according to their answers. If the participants felt one convex surface, d was increased until they could feel two convex surfaces. In contrast, if the participants felt two convex surfaces, d was decreased until they could feel one convex surface. We repeated this process seven times. The first reversal was ignored in subsequent analyses and the remaining six were averaged to obtain an estimate of the discrimination threshold. In this experiment, we had one measure per trial block. The presentation of the different radii was ordered randomly. The boundary of the two convex surfaces remained at the center of the rail (see Fig. 2(a)).
Participants consisted of ten males and two females, with a mean age (± standard deviation) of 25.3 ± 1.2 yrs. All participants had touched the ultrasound in the past, but this was the first time using their fingertips. Before the experiment, subjects were asked to touch the surfaces with the video for them to understand the procedure. Since there were no videos during the experiment, this preparation did not develop the ability to discriminate thresholds well.
3.3 Results
Figure 2(b) and (c) show the experimental results. The discrimination thresholds of each participant obtained by the staircase method were summarized in a box plot for each radius. Among the participants, one participant always felt one convex surface even when the distance was over 50 mm at all radii in the CSC method, and another participant felt both convex surfaces even though the distance was 0 mm \(r_c = 35\) mm of the CSC method. These discrimination thresholds were excluded from the box plots.
Comparing the CPC and CSC methods, we find that the discrimination threshold in the CPC method was generally smaller. In both methods, the smaller the radius of the convex surface, the smaller the discrimination threshold. The minimum threshold throughout the experiment was obtained at \(r_c=5\) mm in the CPC method, with a mean threshold of 8.0 mm and a standard deviation of 2.7 mm. Participants’ comments included: “The CSC method was more difficult to feel,” “Sometimes the number of convex surfaces was judged symbolically,” and “It was more difficult to understand the change from two convex surfaces to one convex surface than from one convex surface to two convex surfaces.” Although we did not specify the speed of the fingers, the measured speed ranged from about 20 to 100 [mm/s].
3.4 Discussion
Comparing the thresholds for both the CPC and CSC methods in each radius, the CPC method had a smaller discrimination threshold than the CSC method at all radii. Therefore, participants were identified the number of convex surfaces more easily using the CPC method. As the authors’ subjective comment, the CPC method provided a clearer sensation of the soft and fine bump profile especially when the height h was lower than about 5 mm (see Fig. 1(c)).
A reason for the lower effectiveness of the CSC method is the weakness of the ultrasonic force (\(1.6\;\;\mathrm {gf/cm}^2\) with 324 transducers [5]). Due to its small maximum strength, it was difficult to understand the difference when the intensity varied. This was also evident in the comments. In particular, when the radius was small in the CSC method, the stimulus occurred at a short horizontal distance, making it more difficult to judge. Therefore, some participants made logical judgments such as whether there was a stimulus difference, rather than report how they felt the curved surface, especially at \(r_c=5\) and 15 mm. Upon improving the presentation power in the future, the CSC method will become more effective and the threshold can be lowered.
However, the CPC method was effective as a method for transmitting mesoscopic features. In particular, the minimum threshold of \(8.0 \pm 2.7\) mm obtained at \(r_c = 5\) mm showed that we can discriminate the spatial pattern on a surface even if the distance d (see Fig. 1(c)) was within the diameter of a finger (about 14 mm) using the CPC method. As the authors’ subjective comment, “the fine profile was naturally perceived though it feels very soft.”
4 Conclusion
In this study, we discussed and evaluated the spatial resolution of mesoscopic shapes created using ultrasonic LM stimulation, in the situation where a finger moves along the surface of a virtual object. As an indicator of the spatial resolution, we used the minimum distance between two bumps by which we can distinguish them.
Both CPC and CSC methods were used to display the surface pattern. In each procedure, the contact position and the force strength were, respectively, controlled according to the finger position. The experimental results show that the CPC method more effectively conveyed mesoscopic features than the CSC method. In addition, it obtained the spatial resolution (minimum discriminable bump distance) of 8.0 mm with a standard deviation of 2.7 mm.
References
Suzuki, Y., Kobayashi, M.: Air jet driven force feedback in virtual reality. IEEE Comput. Graph. Appl. 25(1), 44–47 (2005)
Wang, G.-Z., Huang, Y.-P., et al.: Bare finger 3D air-touch system using an embedded optical sensor array for mobile displays. J. Disp. Technol. 10(1), 13–18 (2014)
Kim, H.-S., Kim, J.-S., et al.: Evaluation of the possibility and response characteristics of laser-induced tactile sensation. Neurosci. Lett. 602, 68–72 (2015)
Iwamoto, T., Tatezono, M., Shinoda, H.: Non-contact method for producing tactile sensation using airborne ultrasound. In: Ferre, M. (ed.) EuroHaptics 2008. LNCS, vol. 5024, pp. 504–513. Springer, Heidelberg (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-69057-3_64
Hoshi, T., Takahashi, M., et al.: Noncontact tactile display based on radiation pressure of airborne ultrasound. IEEE Trans. Haptics 3(3), 155–165 (2010)
Carter, T., Seah, S.A., et al.: UltraHaptics: multi-point midair haptic feedback for touch surfaces. In: Proceedings of 26th Annual ACM Symposium User Interface Software Technology, pp. 505–514 (2013)
Morisaki, T., Fujiwara, M., et al.: Non-vibratory pressure sensation produced by ultrasound focus moving laterally and repetitively with fine spatial step width. IEEE Trans. Haptics (2021)
Takahashi, R., Hasegawa, K., et al.: Tactile stimulation by repetitive lateral movement of midair ultrasound focus. IEEE Trans. Haptics 13(2), 334–342 (2020)
Wijntjes, M.W.A., Sato, A., et al.: Local surface orientation dominates haptic curvature discrimination. IEEE Trans. Haptics 2(2), 94–102 (2009)
Lederman, S.J.: The perception of surface roughness by active and passive touch. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 18(5), 253–255 (1981). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03333619
Howe, R.D., Cutkosky, M.R.: Sensing skin acceleration for slip and texture perception. In: Proceedings of 1989 ICRA, pp. 145–150 (1989)
Matsubayashi, A., Oikawa, H., et al.: Display of haptic shape using ultrasound pressure distribution forming cross-sectional shape. In: Proceedings of IEEE WHC, pp. 419–424 (2019)
Matsubayashi, A., Makino, Y., Shinoda, H.: Rendering ultrasound pressure distribution on hand surface in real-time. In: Nisky, I., Hartcher-O’Brien, J., Wiertlewski, M., Smeets, J. (eds.) EuroHaptics 2020. LNCS, vol. 12272, pp. 407–415. Springer, Cham (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-58147-3_45
Freeman, E., Anderson, R., et al.: Textured surfaces for ultrasound haptic displays. In: Proceedings of ICMI 2017, pp. 491–492 (2017)
Beattie, D., Frier, W., et al.: Incorporating the perception of visual roughness into the design of mid-air haptic textures. In: Proceedings ACM SAP 2020, Article No. 4, pp. 1–10 (2020)
Inoue, S., Makino, Y., et al.: Active touch perception produced by airborne ultrasonic haptic hologram. In: Proceedings of the 2015 IEEE WHC, pp. 362–367 (2015)
Howard, T., Gallagher, G., et al.: Investigating the recognition of local shapes using mid-air ultrasound haptics. In: 2019 IEEE WHC, pp. 503–508 (2019)
Suzuki, S., Inoue, S., et al.: AUTD3: scalable airborne ultrasound tactile display. IEEE Trans. Haptics 14, 740–749 (2021)
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding authors
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Open Access This chapter is licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license and indicate if changes were made.
The images or other third party material in this chapter are included in the chapter's Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the chapter's Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.
Copyright information
© 2022 The Author(s)
About this paper
Cite this paper
Somei, Z., Morisaki, T., Toide, Y., Fujiwara, M., Makino, Y., Shinoda, H. (2022). Spatial Resolution of Mesoscopic Shapes Presented by Airborne Ultrasound. In: Seifi, H., et al. Haptics: Science, Technology, Applications. EuroHaptics 2022. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 13235. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-06249-0_28
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-06249-0_28
Published:
Publisher Name: Springer, Cham
Print ISBN: 978-3-031-06248-3
Online ISBN: 978-3-031-06249-0
eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)