{"status":"ok","message-type":"work","message-version":"1.0.0","message":{"indexed":{"date-parts":[[2024,6,1]],"date-time":"2024-06-01T16:34:47Z","timestamp":1717259687381},"reference-count":39,"publisher":"MIT Press - Journals","issue":"5","content-domain":{"domain":["direct.mit.edu"],"crossmark-restriction":true},"short-container-title":[],"published-print":{"date-parts":[[2014,5,1]]},"abstract":"Abstract<\/jats:title>\n Changes in the sensory properties of numerosity stimuli have a direct effect on the outcomes of nonsymbolic number tasks. This suggests a prominent role of sensory properties in numerosity processing. However, the current consensus holds that numerosity is processed independent of its sensory properties. To investigate the role of sensory cues in ordinal number processes, we manipulated both dimensions orthogonally. Participants passively viewed the stimuli while their brain activity was measured using EEG. The results revealed an interaction between numerosity and its sensory properties in the absence of main effects. Different neural responses were present for trials where numerosity and sensory cues changed in the same direction compared with trials where they changed in opposite directions. These results show that the sensory cues are expected to change in concert with numerosity and support the notion that the visual cues are taken into account when judging numerosity.<\/jats:p>","DOI":"10.1162\/jocn_a_00541","type":"journal-article","created":{"date-parts":[[2013,12,17]],"date-time":"2013-12-17T17:27:18Z","timestamp":1387301238000},"page":"1013-1020","update-policy":"http:\/\/dx.doi.org\/10.1162\/mitpressjournals.corrections.policy","source":"Crossref","is-referenced-by-count":15,"title":["The Neural Mechanism Underlying Ordinal Numerosity Processing"],"prefix":"10.1162","volume":"26","author":[{"given":"Titia","family":"Gebuis","sequence":"first","affiliation":[]},{"given":"Bert","family":"Reynvoet","sequence":"additional","affiliation":[]}],"member":"281","published-online":{"date-parts":[[2014,5,1]]},"reference":[{"key":"2021073000412441000_R1","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"223","DOI":"10.1016\/S0010-0277(02)00005-7","article-title":"The development of ordinal numerical knowledge in infancy.","volume":"83","author":"Brannon","year":"2002","journal-title":"Cognition"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R2","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"B59","DOI":"10.1016\/j.cognition.2004.01.004","article-title":"Number bias for the discrimination of large visual sets in infancy.","volume":"93","author":"Brannon","year":"2004","journal-title":"Cognition"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R3","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"431","DOI":"10.1111\/j.1467-7687.2007.00597.x","article-title":"Heterogeneity impairs numerical matching but not numerical ordering in preschool children.","volume":"10","author":"Cantlon","year":"2007","journal-title":"Developmental Science"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R4","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"307","DOI":"10.1016\/j.neuron.2006.12.025","article-title":"Notation-dependent and -independent representations of numbers in the parietal lobes.","volume":"53","author":"Cohen Kadosh","year":"2007","journal-title":"Neuron"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R5","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"19552","DOI":"10.1073\/pnas.1113195108","article-title":"A common visual metric for approximate number and density.","volume":"108","author":"Dakin","year":"2012","journal-title":"Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A."},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R6","volume-title":"The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics.","author":"Dehaene","year":"1997"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R7","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"307","DOI":"10.1016\/j.tics.2004.05.002","article-title":"Core systems of number.","volume":"8","author":"Feigenson","year":"2004","journal-title":"Trends in Cognitive Sciences"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R8","first-page":"394","article-title":"The development of automated access to symbolic and non-symbolic number knowledge in children: An ERP study.","volume":"48","author":"Gebuis","year":"2009","journal-title":"European Journal of Neuroscience"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R9","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"981","DOI":"10.3758\/s13428-011-0097-5","article-title":"Generating non-symbolic number stimuli.","volume":"43","author":"Gebuis","year":"2011","journal-title":"Behavior Research Methods"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R10","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"1649","DOI":"10.1111\/j.1469-8986.2012.01461.x","article-title":"Continuous visual properties explain neural responses to nonsymbolic number.","volume":"49","author":"Gebuis","year":"2012","journal-title":"Psychophysiology"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R11","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"642","DOI":"10.1037\/a0026218","article-title":"The interplay between nonsymbolic number and its continuous visual properties.","volume":"141","author":"Gebuis","year":"2012","journal-title":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R12","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"e37426","DOI":"10.1371\/journal.pone.0037426","article-title":"The role of visual information in numerosity estimation.","volume":"7","author":"Gebuis","year":"2012","journal-title":"PLoS One"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R13","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"301","DOI":"10.1016\/j.neuroimage.2012.12.048","article-title":"The neural mechanisms underlying passive and active processing of numerosity.","volume":"70C","author":"Gebuis","year":"2013","journal-title":"Neuroimage"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R14","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"e25405","DOI":"10.1371\/journal.pone.0025405","article-title":"False approximations of the approximate number system?","volume":"6","author":"Gebuis","year":"2011","journal-title":"PLoS One"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R15","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"2099","DOI":"10.1080\/17470218.2011.574710","article-title":"Measuring the approximate number system.","volume":"64","author":"Gilmore","year":"2011","journal-title":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: (Colchester)"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R16","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"656","DOI":"10.2466\/pms.1988.67.2.656","article-title":"Perceived numerosity as a function of item size.","volume":"67","author":"Ginsburg","year":"1988","journal-title":"Perceptual and Motor Skills"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R17","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"1536","DOI":"10.1162\/0898929042568442","article-title":"Response-selection-related parietal activation during number comparison.","volume":"16","author":"Gobel","year":"2004","journal-title":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R18","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"665","DOI":"10.1038\/nature07246","article-title":"Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement.","volume":"455","author":"Halberda","year":"2008","journal-title":"Nature"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R19","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"175","DOI":"10.3758\/BF03212070","article-title":"Attention and spatial selection: Electrophysiological evidence for modulation by perceptual load.","volume":"62","author":"Handy","year":"2000","journal-title":"Perception and Psychophysics"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R20","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"1123","DOI":"10.1126\/science.1239052","article-title":"Topographic representation of numerosity in the human parietal cortex.","volume":"341","author":"Harvey","year":"2013","journal-title":"Science"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R21","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"1039","DOI":"10.1162\/jocn.2009.21090","article-title":"All numbers are not equal: An electrophysiological investigation of small and large number representations.","volume":"21","author":"Hyde","year":"2009","journal-title":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R22","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"1222","DOI":"10.3758\/s13423-011-0154-1","article-title":"Non-verbal number acuity correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: But only in children.","volume":"18","author":"Inglis","year":"2011","journal-title":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R23","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"189","DOI":"10.1016\/0926-6410(95)90008-X","article-title":"Luminance and spatial attention effects on early visual processing.","volume":"2","author":"Johannes","year":"1995","journal-title":"Brain Research, Cognitive Brain Research"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R24","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"1","DOI":"10.1186\/1744-9081-3-1","article-title":"Electrophysiological evidence for notation independence in numerical processing.","volume":"3","author":"Libertus","year":"2007","journal-title":"Behavioral and Brain Functions"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R25","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"83","DOI":"10.2307\/1420810","article-title":"The effects of shape, size, heterogeneity, and instructional set on the judgment of visual number.","volume":"81","author":"Miller","year":"1968","journal-title":"American Journal of Psychology"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R26","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"278","DOI":"10.1037\/0033-2909.128.2.278","article-title":"Multiple cues for quantification in infancy: Is number one of them?","volume":"128","author":"Mix","year":"2002","journal-title":"Psychological Bulletin"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R27","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"33","DOI":"10.1016\/j.cognition.2010.03.012","article-title":"Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia.","volume":"116","author":"Piazza","year":"2010","journal-title":"Cognition"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R28","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"547","DOI":"10.1016\/j.neuron.2004.10.014","article-title":"Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus.","volume":"44","author":"Piazza","year":"2004","journal-title":"Neuron"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R29","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"544","DOI":"10.1037\/0012-1649.44.2.544","article-title":"The development of automatic numerosity processing in preschoolers: Evidence for numerosity-perceptual interference.","volume":"44","author":"Rousselle","year":"2008","journal-title":"Developmental Psychology"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R30","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"57","DOI":"10.1016\/j.jecp.2003.10.005","article-title":"Magnitude comparison in preschoolers: What counts? Influence of perceptual variables.","volume":"87","author":"Rousselle","year":"2004","journal-title":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R31","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"13","DOI":"10.1186\/1744-9081-6-13","article-title":"Relationships between magnitude representation, counting and memory in 4- to 7-year-old children: A developmental study.","volume":"6","author":"Soltesz","year":"2010","journal-title":"Behavioral and Brain Functions"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R32","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"460","DOI":"10.1016\/j.cognition.2007.10.009","article-title":"How do people apprehend large numerosities?","volume":"107","author":"Sophian","year":"2008","journal-title":"Cognition"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R33","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"308","DOI":"10.1080\/15250000802188800","article-title":"Changes in the ability to detect ordinal numerical relationships between 9 and 11 months of age.","volume":"13","author":"Suanda","year":"2008","journal-title":"Infancy"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R34","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"143","DOI":"10.1016\/j.brainres.2007.11.010","article-title":"The interaction of task-relevant and task-irrelevant stimulus features in the number\/size congruency paradigm: An ERP study.","volume":"1190","author":"Szucs","year":"2008","journal-title":"Brain Research"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R35","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"23","DOI":"10.1186\/1744-9081-3-23","article-title":"The speed of magnitude processing and executive functions in controlled and automatic number comparison in children: An electro-encephalography study.","volume":"3","author":"Szucs","year":"2007","journal-title":"Behavioral and Brain Functions"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R36","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"22","DOI":"10.1016\/j.cogbrainres.2004.05.003","article-title":"Electrophysiological evidence for differential processing of numerical quantity and order in humans.","volume":"21","author":"Turconi","year":"2004","journal-title":"Brain Research, Cognitive Brain Research"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R37","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"347","DOI":"10.1007\/s00426-007-0108-4","article-title":"Off-line and online automatic number comparison.","volume":"72","author":"Van Opstal","year":"2008","journal-title":"Psychological Research"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R38","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"B1","DOI":"10.1016\/S0010-0277(99)00066-9","article-title":"Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants.","volume":"74","author":"Xu","year":"2000","journal-title":"Cognition"},{"key":"2021073000412441000_R39","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"88","DOI":"10.1111\/j.1467-7687.2005.00395.x","article-title":"Number sense in human infants.","volume":"8","author":"Xu","year":"2005","journal-title":"Developmental Science"}],"container-title":["Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience"],"original-title":[],"language":"en","link":[{"URL":"http:\/\/direct.mit.edu\/jocn\/article-pdf\/26\/5\/1013\/1947272\/jocn_a_00541.pdf","content-type":"application\/pdf","content-version":"vor","intended-application":"syndication"},{"URL":"http:\/\/direct.mit.edu\/jocn\/article-pdf\/26\/5\/1013\/1947272\/jocn_a_00541.pdf","content-type":"unspecified","content-version":"vor","intended-application":"similarity-checking"}],"deposited":{"date-parts":[[2021,7,30]],"date-time":"2021-07-30T00:47:05Z","timestamp":1627606025000},"score":1,"resource":{"primary":{"URL":"https:\/\/direct.mit.edu\/jocn\/article\/26\/5\/1013\/28099\/The-Neural-Mechanism-Underlying-Ordinal-Numerosity"}},"subtitle":[],"short-title":[],"issued":{"date-parts":[[2014,5,1]]},"references-count":39,"journal-issue":{"issue":"5","published-online":{"date-parts":[[2014,5,1]]},"published-print":{"date-parts":[[2014,5,1]]}},"URL":"https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1162\/jocn_a_00541","relation":{},"ISSN":["0898-929X","1530-8898"],"issn-type":[{"value":"0898-929X","type":"print"},{"value":"1530-8898","type":"electronic"}],"subject":[],"published-other":{"date-parts":[[2014,5]]},"published":{"date-parts":[[2014,5,1]]}}}