{"status":"ok","message-type":"work","message-version":"1.0.0","message":{"indexed":{"date-parts":[[2024,8,22]],"date-time":"2024-08-22T20:51:21Z","timestamp":1724359881683},"reference-count":37,"publisher":"Springer Science and Business Media LLC","issue":"4","content-domain":{"domain":[],"crossmark-restriction":false},"short-container-title":["J of Log Lang and Inf"],"published-print":{"date-parts":[[2008,10]]},"DOI":"10.1007\/s10849-008-9065-6","type":"journal-article","created":{"date-parts":[[2008,6,30]],"date-time":"2008-06-30T13:14:52Z","timestamp":1214831692000},"page":"443-466","source":"Crossref","is-referenced-by-count":21,"title":["Age Differences in Adults\u2019 Use of Referring Expressions"],"prefix":"10.1007","volume":"17","author":[{"given":"Petra","family":"Hendriks","sequence":"first","affiliation":[]},{"given":"Christina","family":"Englert","sequence":"additional","affiliation":[]},{"given":"Ellis","family":"Wubs","sequence":"additional","affiliation":[]},{"given":"John","family":"Hoeks","sequence":"additional","affiliation":[]}],"member":"297","published-online":{"date-parts":[[2008,7,1]]},"reference":[{"key":"9065_CR1","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"202","DOI":"10.1006\/brln.1999.2055","volume":"67","author":"A. Almor","year":"1999","unstructured":"Almor A., Kempler D., MacDonald M.C., Andersen E.S., Tyler L.K. (1999) Why do Alzheimer patients have difficulty with pronouns? Working memory, semantics, and reference in comprehension and production in Alzheimer\u2019s disease. Brain and Language 67: 202\u2013227","journal-title":"Brain and Language"},{"key":"9065_CR2","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"65","DOI":"10.1017\/S0022226700011567","volume":"24","author":"M. Ariel","year":"1988","unstructured":"Ariel M. (1988) Referring and accessibility. Journal of Linguistics 24: 65\u201387","journal-title":"Journal of Linguistics"},{"key":"9065_CR3","volume-title":"Anaphoric antecedents","author":"M. Ariel","year":"1990","unstructured":"Ariel M. (1990) Anaphoric antecedents. Croom Helm, London"},{"key":"9065_CR4","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"189","DOI":"10.1093\/jos\/17.3.189","volume":"17","author":"R. Blutner","year":"2000","unstructured":"Blutner R. (2000) Some aspects of optimality in natural language interpretation. Journal of Semantics 17: 189\u2013216","journal-title":"Journal of Semantics"},{"key":"9065_CR5","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","unstructured":"Blutner, R. (2007). Optimality theoretic pragmatics and the explicature\/implicature distinction. In N. Burton-Roberts (Ed.), Advances in pragmatics (pp. 67\u201389). Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire: Palgrave\/MacMillan.","DOI":"10.1057\/978-1-349-73908-0_5"},{"key":"9065_CR6","volume-title":"Optimal communication","author":"R. Blutner","year":"2006","unstructured":"Blutner R., de Hoop H., Hendriks P. (2006) Optimal communication. CSLI Publications, Stanford"},{"key":"9065_CR7","unstructured":"Blutner, R, & Zeevat, H. (2004). Editors\u2019 introduction: Pragmatics in Optimality Theory. In R. Blutner, & H. Zeevat (Eds.), Optimality theory and pragmatics (pp. 1\u201324). Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire: Palgrave\/Macmillan."},{"key":"9065_CR8","unstructured":"Bouma, G. J. (2007). Starting a sentence in Dutch: A corpus study of subject- and object-fronting. Manuscript, University of Groningen."},{"key":"9065_CR9","first-page":"93","volume-title":"Is the best good enough? Optimality and competition in syntax.","author":"L. Burzio","year":"1998","unstructured":"Burzio L. (1998) Anaphora and soft constraints. In: Barbosa P., Fox D., Hagstrom P., McGinnis M., Pesetsky D.(eds) Is the best good enough? Optimality and competition in syntax.. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 93\u2013113"},{"key":"9065_CR10","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"225","DOI":"10.1207\/s15327817la0103_2","volume":"13","author":"Y.-C. Chien","year":"1990","unstructured":"Chien Y.-C., Wexler K. (1990) Children\u2019s knowledge of locality conditions on binding as evidence for the modularity of syntax and pragmatics. Language Acquisition 13: 225\u2013295","journal-title":"Language Acquisition"},{"key":"9065_CR11","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"62","DOI":"10.1016\/j.jml.2003.08.004","volume":"50","author":"H.H. Clark","year":"2004","unstructured":"Clark H.H., Krych M.A. (2004) Speaking while monitoring addressees for understanding. Journal of Memory and Language 50: 62\u201381","journal-title":"Journal of Memory and Language"},{"key":"9065_CR12","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"1","DOI":"10.1016\/0010-0277(86)90010-7","volume":"22","author":"H.H. Clark","year":"1986","unstructured":"Clark H.H., Wilkes-Gibbs D. (1986) Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition 22: 1\u201339","journal-title":"Cognition"},{"key":"9065_CR13","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"37","DOI":"10.1177\/001316446002000104","volume":"20","author":"J. Cohen","year":"1960","unstructured":"Cohen J. (1960) A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement 20: 37\u201346","journal-title":"Educational and Psychological Measurement"},{"key":"9065_CR14","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","unstructured":"De Hoop, H., Kr\u00e4mer, I. (2005\/2006). Children\u2019s optimal interpretations of indefinite subjects and objects. Language Acquisition, 13, 103\u2013123.","DOI":"10.1207\/s15327817la1302_4"},{"key":"9065_CR15","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"760","DOI":"10.1016\/j.jesp.2004.02.002","volume":"40","author":"N. Epley","year":"2004","unstructured":"Epley N., Morewedge C.K., Keysar B. (2004) Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40: 760\u2013768","journal-title":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology"},{"key":"9065_CR16","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","DOI":"10.1075\/z.engram1","volume-title":"English grammar: A function-based introduction","author":"T. Giv\u00f3n","year":"1993","unstructured":"Giv\u00f3n T. (1993) English grammar: A function-based introduction. John Benjamins, Amsterdam"},{"key":"9065_CR17","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","unstructured":"Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole, & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics (pp. 41\u201358). Vol. 3, Academic Press.","DOI":"10.1163\/9789004368811_003"},{"key":"9065_CR18","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"274","DOI":"10.2307\/416535","volume":"69","author":"J. Gundel","year":"1993","unstructured":"Gundel J., Hedberg N., Zacharski R. (1993) Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language 69: 274\u2013307","journal-title":"Language"},{"key":"9065_CR19","unstructured":"Hendriks, P., & Spenader, J. (2004). A bidirectional explanation of the pronoun interpretation problem. In P. Schlenker, & E. Keenan (Eds.), Proceedings of the ESSLLI \u201904 Workshop on Semantic Approaches to Binding Theory. Nancy, France."},{"key":"9065_CR20","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","unstructured":"Hendriks, P., & Spenader, J. (2005\/2006). When production precedes comprehension: An optimization approach to the acquisition of pronouns. Language Acquisition, 13, 319\u2013348.","DOI":"10.1207\/s15327817la1304_3"},{"key":"9065_CR21","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"1879","DOI":"10.1016\/j.lingua.2006.11.008","volume":"117","author":"P. Hendriks","year":"2007","unstructured":"Hendriks P., van Rijn H., Valkenier B. (2007) Learning to reason about speakers\u2019 alternatives in sentence comprehension: A computational account. Lingua 117: 1879\u20131896","journal-title":"Lingua"},{"key":"9065_CR22","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"251","DOI":"10.1057\/9780230501409_11","volume-title":"Optimality theory and pragmatics","author":"G. J\u00e4ger","year":"2004","unstructured":"J\u00e4ger G. (2004) Learning constraint sub-hierarchies: The bidirectional gradual learning algorithm. In: Blutner R., Zeevat H.(eds) Optimality theory and pragmatics. Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire, Palgrave\/Macmillan, pp 251\u2013287"},{"key":"9065_CR23","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"61","DOI":"10.1080\/01690968508402071","volume":"1","author":"A. Karmiloff-Smith","year":"1985","unstructured":"Karmiloff-Smith A. (1985) Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Language and Cognitive Processes 1: 61\u201385","journal-title":"Language and Cognitive Processes"},{"key":"9065_CR24","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"P350","DOI":"10.1093\/geronb\/54B.6.P350","volume":"B54","author":"K.A. Kemtes","year":"1999","unstructured":"Kemtes K.A., Kemper S. (1999) Aging and resolution of quantifier scope. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences B54: P350\u2013P360","journal-title":"Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences"},{"key":"9065_CR25","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"71","DOI":"10.1515\/IP.2007.004","volume":"4","author":"B. Keysar","year":"2007","unstructured":"Keysar B. (2007) Communication and miscommunication: The role of egocentric processes. Intercultural Pragmatics 4: 71\u201384","journal-title":"Intercultural Pragmatics"},{"key":"9065_CR26","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"46","DOI":"10.1111\/1467-8721.ep13175613","volume":"7","author":"B. Keysar","year":"1998","unstructured":"Keysar B., Barr D.J., Horton W.S. (1998) The egocentric basis of language use: Insights from a processing approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science 7: 46\u201350","journal-title":"Current Directions in Psychological Science"},{"key":"9065_CR27","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"159","DOI":"10.2307\/2529310","volume":"33","author":"J.R. Landis","year":"1977","unstructured":"Landis J.R., Koch G.G. (1977) The measure of observer agreement for categorial data. Biometrics 33: 159\u2013174","journal-title":"Biometrics"},{"key":"9065_CR28","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"403","DOI":"10.1017\/S0142716406060334","volume":"27","author":"D. Matthews","year":"2006","unstructured":"Matthews D., Lieven E., Theakston A., Tomasello M. (2006) The effect of perceptual availability and prior discourse on young children\u2019s use of referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics 27: 403\u2013422","journal-title":"Applied Psycholinguistics"},{"key":"9065_CR29","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","unstructured":"Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (2004). Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Blackwell. Also appeared as Technical Report CU-CS-696-93, Department of Computer Science, University of Colorado at Boulder, and Technical Report TR-2, Rutgers Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ, April 1993.","DOI":"10.1002\/9780470756171.ch1"},{"key":"9065_CR30","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"109","DOI":"10.1207\/s15327817la1202_1","volume":"12","author":"T. Reinhart","year":"2004","unstructured":"Reinhart T. (2004) The processing cost of reference-set computation: Acquisition of stress shift and focus. Language Acquisition 12: 109\u2013155","journal-title":"Language Acquisition"},{"key":"9065_CR31","unstructured":"Reinhart, T. (to appear). Processing or pragmatics? Explaining the coreference delay. In T. Gibson and N. Pearlmutter (Eds.), The processing and acquisition of reference. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press."},{"key":"9065_CR32","volume-title":"Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences","author":"J. Stevens","year":"1992","unstructured":"Stevens J. (1992) Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum"},{"key":"9065_CR33","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","DOI":"10.7551\/mitpress\/5550.001.0001","volume-title":"Principle B, VP ellipsis and interpretation in child grammar","author":"R. Thornton","year":"1999","unstructured":"Thornton R., Wexler K. (1999) Principle B, VP ellipsis and interpretation in child grammar. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA"},{"key":"9065_CR34","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"465","DOI":"10.7551\/mitpress\/5161.003.0018","volume-title":"Optimality-theoretic syntax","author":"C. Wilson","year":"2001","unstructured":"Wilson C. (2001) Bidirectional optimization and the theory of anaphora. In: Legendre G., Grimshaw J., Vikner S.(eds) Optimality-theoretic syntax. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 465\u2013507"},{"key":"9065_CR35","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"103","DOI":"10.1080\/03610738808259731","volume":"14","author":"A. Wingfield","year":"1988","unstructured":"Wingfield A., Stine E.A.L., Lahar C.J., Aberdeen J.S. (1988) Does the capacity of working memory change with age?. Experimental Aging Research 14: 103\u2013107","journal-title":"Experimental Aging Research"},{"key":"9065_CR36","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"541","DOI":"10.1017\/S0142716405050290","volume":"26","author":"A. Wittek","year":"2005","unstructured":"Wittek A., Tomasello M. (2005) Young children\u2019s sensitivity to listener knowledge and perceptual context in choosing referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics 26: 541\u2013558","journal-title":"Applied Psycholinguistics"},{"key":"9065_CR37","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","first-page":"243","DOI":"10.1093\/jos\/17.3.243","volume":"17","author":"H. Zeevat","year":"2000","unstructured":"Zeevat H. (2000) The asymmetry of optimality theoretic syntax and semantics. Journal of Semantics 17: 243\u2013262","journal-title":"Journal of Semantics"}],"container-title":["Journal of Logic, Language and Information"],"original-title":[],"language":"en","link":[{"URL":"http:\/\/link.springer.com\/content\/pdf\/10.1007\/s10849-008-9065-6.pdf","content-type":"unspecified","content-version":"vor","intended-application":"similarity-checking"}],"deposited":{"date-parts":[[2024,2,27]],"date-time":"2024-02-27T23:27:47Z","timestamp":1709076467000},"score":1,"resource":{"primary":{"URL":"http:\/\/link.springer.com\/10.1007\/s10849-008-9065-6"}},"subtitle":[],"short-title":[],"issued":{"date-parts":[[2008,7,1]]},"references-count":37,"journal-issue":{"issue":"4","published-print":{"date-parts":[[2008,10]]}},"alternative-id":["9065"],"URL":"https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1007\/s10849-008-9065-6","relation":{},"ISSN":["0925-8531","1572-9583"],"issn-type":[{"value":"0925-8531","type":"print"},{"value":"1572-9583","type":"electronic"}],"subject":[],"published":{"date-parts":[[2008,7,1]]}}}